Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Potential mechanisms underlying sleep disturbance in young people with borderline personality disorder features: an exploratory study

Abstract

Background

Sleep disturbance is common among young people (15–25 years) with features of borderline personality disorder (BPD). However, the mechanisms underlying sleep disturbance in BPD remain unknown. Understanding these underlying processes is essential to guide the development of sleep-improvement interventions and to optimise their efficacy through identifying beneficial treatment targets. This exploratory study aimed to investigate potential underlying mechanisms to inform future hypotheses, research development, and provide insight into potential treatment targets to improve sleep in young people with BPD. This study explored the indirect roles of emotion regulation difficulties, depression, anxiety and stress in the relationship between BPD features and sleep disturbance in young people.

Methods

Sleep was measured subjectively (self-report questionnaires) and objectively (10 days wrist actigraphy) in 40 young people with BPD features and 38 healthy young people. Participants also completed the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale and the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale.

Results

Mediation analyses revealed that impulse control difficulties, limited emotion regulation strategies and anxiety indirectly affected the relationship between group (BPD vs. healthy) and subjective sleep disturbance in young people. Lack of emotional awareness and anxiety contributed to associations between group and objectively longer time in bed and bedtime variability, respectively.

Conclusions

These preliminary findings suggest that targeting emotional dysregulation (impulse control, strategies, emotional awareness) and anxiety might be beneficial for improving sleep in this population.

Background

Sleep problems are common in young people (aged 15–25 years) with features of borderline personality disorder (BPD), a severe mental disorder characterised by unstable interpersonal relationships, emotions and self-image, and impulsive behaviours [3, 41, 42]. Compared with healthy young people, individuals with BPD features report poorer subjective sleep quality and display objectively longer, later and more irregular sleep timing [38, 41, 40, 75]. However, the mechanisms underlying the relationship between BPD features and sleep problems remain largely unknown. Understanding these underlying processes is essential to guide the development of sleep-improvement interventions and to optimise their efficacy through identifying beneficial treatment targets. This study focussed on emotion regulation difficulties, depression, anxiety and stress, each of which are common in young people with BPD and independently associated with sleep disturbance.

Emotion regulation difficulties

Emotional dysregulation, a defining feature of BPD [19, 26], is bidirectionally associated with sleep disturbance [27, 61]. That is, emotion regulation difficulties disturb sleep, through heightened arousal and negative affect, while sleep problems exacerbate emotional dysregulation [6, 21, 27, 34, 63]. Young people, whose emotion regulation skills are still developing, might be particularly susceptible to the emotional effects of poor sleep [63]. Nightmares, which are common in individuals with BPD, can also exacerbate daytime emotional dysregulation [68] and might even increase the risk of developing BPD by exacerbating trauma responses [79]. Therefore, understanding the relationships between BPD features, emotional dysregulation, and sleep problems during this developmental period is crucial.

Two studies have explored the role of emotional dysregulation in sleep disturbance in young people with BPD features [29, 75]. Grove et al. [29] found that global emotion regulation difficulties contributed to the relationship between BPD features and subjective sleep disturbance in university undergraduates. Subsequent research in a clinical sample, however, observed no effect of emotion regulation difficulties in this relationship and highlighted the need for additional research that includes objective sleep measures [75].

Grove et al. [29] also explored specific aspects of emotional dysregulation. Only lack of emotional awareness and clarity, and limited access to emotion regulation strategies contributed to the relationship between BPD features and subjective sleep disturbance [29]. These findings warrant replication and clarification in a clinical sample as it remains unclear why only these aspects of emotional dysregulation were found to contribute to sleep disturbance in BPD [29].

Depression, anxiety and stress

The null findings from Wall et al. [75] suggest that other factors related to emotional dysregulation, such as depression, anxiety, or stress, might underlie sleep problems in this population. Depression, anxiety, and stress commonly co-occur with BPD in young people [12, 78] and are independently associated with sleep disturbance [2, 57, 64]. Although sleep problems in BPD cannot be solely attributed to co-occurring depression, the two disorders share similar sleep profiles [30, 59, 79]. As such, depressive symptoms might still contribute to sleep problems in individuals with BPD features, albeit indirectly.

Pre-sleep arousal is a key contributor to sleep disturbance, and specifically exacerbates subjective sleep problems [33, 71]. Yet, anxiety and stress have been largely neglected as potential underlying mechanisms in the field of sleep and BPD [79]. Pre-sleep arousal might be further exacerbated by nightmares in individuals with BPD features, who report heightened dream anxiety and fear of falling asleep due to anticipating a nightmare [69]. Anxiety and stress-related arousal, combined with heightened baseline arousal in BPD [48, 68], might contribute to sleep disturbance in this population.

To our knowledge, no studies have explored the roles of depression, anxiety and stress alongside emotional dysregulation, which is crucial given that these factors are interrelated [24, 45].

The current study

This exploratory study aimed to investigate potential underlying mechanisms to inform future hypotheses and research development. It explored the roles of emotional dysregulation, depression, anxiety and stress in the sleep profile of young people with BPD features. It aimed to extend previous research by using a combination of subjective and objective (actigraphy) sleep measures, by investigating emotional dysregulation, and by exploring the comparative strengths of these potential mechanisms.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 78 young people (aged 15–25 years), 40 with BPD features (36 female, Mage = 19.77, SDage = 2.51) and 38 healthy young people (34 female, Mage = 20.06, SDage = 2.52). This age range represents a developmentally coherent group [11, 14], and a previous study using the same sample revealed no significant differences between adolescents (aged 15–17 years) and young adults (aged 18–25 years) with BPD features [42]. Within the BPD group, eleven (27.5%) participants had sub-threshold BPD (3–4 features) and 24 (60%) were taking prescribed psychotropic medication. By design, t here were no significant group differences in age or socioeconomic status, and sex distributions were comparable across groups to help eliminate any potential confounding effects of these factors on sleep. See [42] for detailed sample characteristics.

Participants with BPD features were recruited through the Helping Young People Early (HYPE) Program [13] at Orygen, the government-funded specialist mental health service for young people (aged 15–25 years) residing in western metropolitan Melbourne, Australia. Participants had three or more features of BPD, assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) Personality Disorders (SCID-5-PD). Individuals with 3–4 BPD features were included, given the clinical significance and comparable functional impairment of individuals with sub-threshold BPD to those who meet full diagnostic criteria [47, 72, 80]. Exclusion criteria included a current DSM-5 diagnosis of schizophrenia spectrum disorder or bipolar I disorder, or a diagnosis of sleep apnea or a score above 0.5 on the multivariable apnoea risk index (MAPI [51];).

Healthy participants were recruited via word of mouth and social media (Facebook and Instagram). They had zero SCID-5-PD assessed BPD features, had never received any mental health treatment or been diagnosed with any mental or sleep disorders, and had a score below 0.5 on the MAPI.

The current study adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by Melbourne Health Human Research Ethics Committee (#2017.153).

Measures

Predictor variable

Group status was used as the predictor variable wherein 0 indicated a healthy participant and 1 indicated a participant with BPD features. Diagnoses were assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5, Research Version (SCID-5-RV: [22]) and SCID-5-PD [23]. Healthy individuals did not complete the SCID-5-RV and only completed the BPD module of the SCID-5-PD. Healthy participants were instead asked to report any mental disorder diagnoses (past or present) and any history of help-seeking (eg. seeing a school counsellor or psychologist).

Mediating variables

Difficulties in emotion regulation (DERS)

The DERS is a 36-item self-report questionnaire with high internal consistency (⍺ = 0.93) and good test-retest reliability (ρI = 88) [28]. Items are rated on a scale of 1 (“almost never [0–10%]”) to 5 (“almost always [91–100%]”), where higher scores represent greater difficulties in emotion regulation. The DERS yields a global score and six subscales: nonacceptance, goals, impulse, awareness, strategies, and clarity.

Depression anxiety and stress scales (DASS) 21

The DASS-21 is a 21-item self-report measure [49]. Each item is rated from 0 (never) to 3 (almost always), where higher scores indicate greater distress. The DASS includes three subscales, each with good internal consistency: depression (⍺ = 0.91), anxiety (⍺ = 0.84), and stress (⍺ = 0.90).

Outcome variables

Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI)

The PSQI is a 9-item self-report measure of global sleep quality over the previous month [8], with good internal consistency (α = 0.87 [4]). It has seven component scores: sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleeping medications, and daytime dysfunction. Each component is scored from 0 (no difficulty) to 3 (severe difficulty). A global score is calculated by summing the seven components, where higher scores represent poorer sleep quality. The global score was used in the current study.

Insomnia severity index (ISI)

The ISI is a 7-item measure of an individual’s clinical level of insomnia over the past 2 weeks [54]. The ISI has high internal consistency, α = 0.90 [55]. The index addresses difficulties initiating and maintaining sleep, sleep-related satisfaction and worry/distress, and the functional impact of insomnia symptoms. Each item is rated on a scale from 0 to 4 and a total score is calculated by summing all ratings, where higher scores reflected more severe insomnia.

Epworth sleepiness scale (ESS)

The ESS is an 8-item measure of an individual’s general level of daytime sleepiness [43], with good internal consistency, α = 0.88 [44]. Individuals rate their chances of dozing off or falling asleep while engaged in eight different activities on a scale of 0 (never) to 3 (high chance). A total sleepiness score is calculated by summing the eight ratings.

Consensus sleep diary

This sleep diary is a widely used and validated 15-item self-report measure [9, 50]. The diary assesses sleep timing, quality and behavioural factors that might affect sleep (eg. caffeine and alcohol intake, napping, medication use). Only bed and rise times from the diary were used in the current study, to inform the sleep period for actigraphy analyses.

Actigraphy

A GENEActiv® Original (ActivInsights Ltd., Cambridge, UK) triaxial accelerometry-based activity monitor was used to provide activity counts (per minute), configured with a sampling frequency of 50 Hz. GENEActiv data stored in 60-s epochs were downloaded for analysis using GENEActiv PC software (Activinsights; version 3.2). Data for all participants were visually checked against sleep diary data to confirm bed and rise times. The Philips Respironics algorithm [58] with a wake threshold of 115 was used to calculate sleep parameters outlined in Table 1. Research supports use of this algorithm and device-specific wake threshold (see Jenkins, Tiley et al., [40] for more information).

Table 1 Objective Sleep Parameter Definitions

Procedure

Informed consent was obtained from all participants and from a parent or guardian for participants aged under 18 years. Following screening for inclusion/exclusion criteria, participants completed the psychopathology interviews (SCID-5-RV; SCID-5-PD) and self-report questionnaires. Participants were provided with a GENEActiv device which they were instructed to wear continuously on their non-dominant wrist for the following 10 days. Participants were also asked to complete the sleep diary each day while wearing the device. Data collection took place during the educational term/semester (not during vacation or break periods) for individuals engaged in education at the time of participation, to ensure that any observed sleep disturbances were not attributable to being on break/off schedule.

Data analysis

Bed and rise times were converted to minutes for analysis. Midnight was treated as zero, such that times before midnight were converted to negative values (eg. 11:30 pm = −30mins) and times after midnight were converted to positive values (eg. 3:00 am = 180mins).

All aims were tested via mediation analyses, using non-parametric bootstrapping in PROCESS (Model 4, [36]). PROCESS is a widely used SPSS macro developed to conduct mediation and moderation analyses [36]. Indirect effects were considered significant if 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (BCa CI), bootstrapped on 5000 resamples, did not contain zero. Across all models, the independent variable was a binary group variable, where 0 indicated a healthy participant and 1 indicated a participant with BPD features. Sleep was the dependent variable and separate mediation models were analysed for each sleep parameter (PSQI, ISI, ESS, actigraphy variables). Correlations between group, mediating and outcome variables were assessed prior to mediation analyses.

Separate simple mediation analyses examined the indirect effect of each DERS subscales as well as DERS total score in the relationship between BPD and sleep disturbance. If significant indirect effects were observed for more than one DERS subscale, a parallel multiple mediation model was analysed (including all six DERS subscales as parallel mediators) to determine which had the strongest effect. Parallel multiple mediation is beneficial in comparing competing theories and determining which is the strongest mediator, as it provides specific indirect effects for each mediator while controlling for all other mediators by holding scores constant [36]. Conceptual diagrams for simple and parallel mediation models are provided in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Conceptual Diagrams of Simple and Parallel Mediation Models. Note. X = independent variable. Y = dependent variable. M = mediating variable

A similar step-down approach was used to examine the roles of depression, anxiety and stress. First, separate simple mediation analyses investigated the indirect effects of the three DASS subscales. If simple mediation analyses indicated significant indirect effects of more than one subscale, a multiple mediation analysis was conducted including depression, anxiety and stress as parallel mediators.

Finally, for any sleep parameters that had significant indirect effects of one or more DERS subscale and one or more DASS subscale in the simple mediation analyses outlined above, multiple mediation models (including all DERS and DASS subscales as parallel mediators) were analysed. Further, if the DERS total score and one or more DASS subscale displayed significant indirect effects in the simple mediation analyses, another multiple mediation analysis was conducted (including DERS total score and the three DASS subscales as parallel mediators). Pairwise comparisons between significant indirect effects were conducted for all multiple mediation models to determine whether effects significantly differed from one another.

Results

PSQI, ISI, ESS and DASS data were collected and analysable from all participants. DERS data were not available for six participants in the BPD group. Actigraphy data were not analysable for four females (3 BPD, 1 healthy comparison); one due to a failure to return the actigraph, one due to dermal irritation, and two due to insufficient wear time (one and two days, respectively).

Final sample sizes were 78 (40 BPD, 38 HC) for subjective sleep and DASS analyses; 72 (34 BPC, 38 HC) for subjective sleep and DERS analyses; 74 (37 BPCD, 37 HC) for objective sleep and DASS analyses; and 68 (31 BPD, 37 HC) for objective sleep and DERS analyses.

Data regarding subjective chronotype (an individual’s natural inclination towards morning or evening preference for activity) is provided in Supplementary Material (see Table S1 and Fig. S1).

Descriptive statistics and correlations

Group means and standard deviations for all variables and are displayed in Table 2. As this study was exploratory, independent samples t-tests (BPD vs. healthy comparison) were conducted for all variables, see Table 2.

Table 2 Mean (Standard Deviation) and Independent Sample t-test Results for Depression, Anxiety, Stress, Emotion Regulation, and Sleep Measures Across BPD and Healthy Compasison Groups

Correlations between group, mediating and outcome variables are displayed in Table 3. Several outcome variables were not significantly correlated with group or any mediating variables and were therefore not included in the mediation analyses. These were: daytime sleepiness (ESS), bedtime, sleep onset latency, sleep efficiency, wake after sleep onset, time in bed variability and total sleep time variability. These variables also did not significantly differ between BPD and healthy comparison groups (see Table 2).

Table 3 Pearson and Point-Biserial Correlations Between Group, Mediating and Outcome Variables

Subjective sleep

Emotion regulation

Simple mediation analyses indicated independent indirect effects of DERS total score, and impulse and strategies subscales in the relationship between group status and poor subjective sleep quality. Similarly, significant indirect effects of the impulse and strategies subscales were observed for subjective insomnia severity. See Table 4.

Table 4 Simple Mediation Analyses

Multiple mediation models including all DERS subscales as parallel mediators were analysed for subjective sleep quality and insomnia severity. For sleep quality, the overall indirect effect was significant (b = 6.18, 95% BCa CI [2.77, 8.97]), however only the indirect effect of the impulse subscale remained significant (b = 4.27, 95% BCa CI [1.07, 7.58]), see Fig. 2A.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Indirect Roles of Emotion Regulation Difficulties, Depression, Anxiety and Stress in the Relationship Between Group Membership and Subjective Sleep Quality. Note. Solid lines and bolded values represent significant associations, based on 95% bias-corrected bootstrap 95% confidence intervals not including zero. Dashed lines represent non-significant associations. HC = healthy comparison. BPD = borderline personality disorder

The overall indirect effect for insomnia severity was also significant (b = 11.02, 95% BCa CI [4.59, 16.33]), whilst the indirect effects of the impulse (b = 5.30, 95% BCa CI [0.27, 10.16]) and strategies (b = 8.00, 95% BCa CI [0.27, 15.70]) subscales both remained significant, see Fig. 3A. Pairwise comparisons revealed that the indirect effects of the impulse and strategies subscales were not significantly different (b = − 2.69, 95% BCa CI [− 13.00, 7.53]).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Indirect Roles of Emotion Regulation Difficulties, Depression, Anxiety and Stress in the Relationship Between Group Membership and Subjective Insomnia Severity. Note. Solid lines and bolded values represent significant associations, based on 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals not including zero. Dashed lines represent non-significant associations. HC = healthy comparison. BPD = borderline personality disorder

Depression, anxiety and stress

Simple mediation analyses revealed independent indirect effects of depression, anxiety and stress, in the relationships between BPD and subjective sleep quality and insomnia (Table 4). Multiple mediation models including depression, anxiety and stress as parallel mediators were analysed for subjective sleep quality and insomnia severity. For subjective sleep quality the overall indirect effect was significant (b = 4.45, 95% BCa CI [1.86, 6.59]), however only the indirect effect of anxiety remained significant (b = 3.23, 95% BCa CI [0.15, 5.97]) (Fig. 2B).

Similarly, the overall indirect effect for insomnia was significant (b = 7.38, 95% BCa CI [2.54, 11.41]), and only anxiety displayed a significant effect (b = 4.92, 95% BCa CI [0.38, 9.60]) (Fig. 3B).

Emotional regulation, depression, anxiety, and stress

Multiple mediation models, with all DERS and DASS subscales included as parallel mediators, were analysed for subjective sleep quality and insomnia severity. A second multiple mediation model, including DERS total, depression, anxiety, stress as parallel mediators, was analysed for subjective sleep quality.

Regarding sleep quality, the first multiple mediation model revealed a significant total indirect effect (b = 7.17, 95% BCa CI [3.80, 9.95]), and the indirect effects of the impulse (b = 0.58, 95% BCa CI [1.32, 8.37]) and anxiety (b = 4.43, 95% BCa CI [0.53, 7.74]) subscales remained significant (Fig. 2C). Pairwise comparisons revealed that the indirect effects of the impulse and anxiety subscales were not significantly different (b = 0.11, 95% BCa CI [− 0.93, 1.15]).

The second multiple mediation model indicated a significant total indirect effect (b = 4.78, 95% BCa CI [1.27, 7.64]), however only the indirect effect of anxiety remained significant (b = 3.64, 95% BCa CI [0.33, 6.69]) (Fig. 2D).

Regarding insomnia severity, the multiple mediation model revealed a significant total indirect effect (b = 12.41, 95% BCa CI [6.09, 17.66]).The indirect effects of the impulse (b = 5.44, 95% BCa CI [0.13, 10.95]) and anxiety (b = 5.98, 95% BCa CI [0.69, 10.69]) subscales remained significant (Fig. 3C). Pairwise comparisons revealed that the indirect effects of the impulse and anxiety subscales were not significantly different (b = − 0.55, 95% BCa CI [− 7.22, 6.39]).

Objective sleep

Emotion regulation

Table 4 shows that there was an indirect effect of the awareness subscale in the relationship between BPD features and time in bed. No indirect effects of emotion regulation difficulties were observed for the remaining objective sleep parameters.

Depression, anxiety and stress

There was an indirect effect of anxiety in the relationship between BPD features and bedtime variability (Table 4). No indirect effects of depression, anxiety or stress were observed for the remaining objective sleep parameters.

Discussion

This exploratory study aimed to investigate potential underlying mechanisms to inform future hypotheses and research development. It explored the roles of emotional dysregulation, depression, anxiety and stress in the relationship between BPD features and sleep disturbance in young people. Global difficulties in emotional regulation, impulse control difficulties, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, depression, anxiety and stress all indirectly contributed to the relationship between BPD features and subjective sleep disturbance. Further, lack of emotional awareness and anxiety contributed to the associations between BPD features and objectively longer time in bed and bedtime variability, respectively.

Subjective sleep

The current findings align with extant research indicating that subjective sleep disturbance is associated with emotional dysregulation [27, 61], maladaptive coping strategies [35], impulsivity [5, 62, 73], depression, anxiety, and stress [2, 57, 64]. Notably, global emotion regulation difficulties contributed to poor global sleep quality, but not to insomnia severity specifically. This might help to explain inconsistencies in previous research, where one study used a general measure of sleep quality [29] while another focussed on more specific aspects of sleep such as sleep/wake behaviour problems [75].

The findings partially supported previous research. However, the effect of impulse control difficulties was unexpected [29]. Impulse control difficulties increase with BPD severity [16] and Grove et al.’s [29] student sample had less severe BPD pathology than is typically observed in clinical populations. This might help to explain why an effect of impulse control difficulties was observed in the current study, but not in Grove et al. [29]. Moreover, no effects of the DERS nonacceptance and clarity subscales were observed, contradicting previous reports [29]. Again, it is possible that differences in BPD severity played a role in these discrepant findings. However, as disorder severity was not assessed in the current study, this suggestion remains speculative and warrants further investigation.

The key mechanisms underlying subjective sleep disturbance in young people with BPD features were impulse control difficulties, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and anxiety. These factors are interrelated [65, 66] but are independently associated with heightened arousal [20, 49, 67, 76, 77]. Cognitive and physiological arousal exacerbate subjective sleep disturbance and individuals often attribute their sleep problems to uncontrollable cognitive arousal [33, 71]. Specifically, control-related cognitive activity, and an external locus of control, wherein individuals believe that their circumstances are due to external forces beyond their control, perpetuate sleep disturbance through fuelling sleep-related worry and arousal [31, 33, 70]. External locus of control and control-related thoughts are reflected in the impulse and strategies DERS subscales [37, 53]. For example, “I experience my emotions as overwhelming and out of control” (impulse) and “When I’m upset, I believe there is nothing I can do to make myself feel better” (strategies). Locus of control has been identified as a sleep treatment target in other mental disorders [17] and can be modified through cognitive behavioural intervention [74].

Impulse control difficulties might also be related to disturbed sleep through greater pre-sleep smartphone use in young people with BPD features. Smartphone dependence has been associated with impulse control difficulties (Chóliz, [18]), and pre-sleep smartphone use is known to disturb sleep quality and duration through increased physiological and cognitive arousal [32]. Impulse control difficulties in young people with BPD features might therefore result in greater difficulty switching off one’s smartphone to go to sleep. As the current study did not assess smartphone use, this suggestion remains speculative and warrants further investigation.

Overall, these preliminary findings suggest that addressing impulse control, emotion regulation strategies and anxiety through locus of control and pre-sleep arousal might be beneficial in improving subjective sleep in young people with BPD features. However, longitudinal data is needed to support these suggestions.

Objective sleep

Emotional dysregulation, depression, anxiety or stress did not indirectly affect any objective sleep parameters other than longer time in bed and bedtime variability. This is likely because few objective sleep disturbances have been observed in young people with BPD features [38, 41, 42].

Lack of emotional awareness and longer time in bed

Lack of emotional awareness (eg. “I do [not] pay attention to how I feel”) might reflect a maladaptive coping strategy of emotional avoidance. Spending more time in bed can serve as a similar emotionally avoidant coping strategy [24, 39, 46], which might explain the relationship between these factors. Crucially, emotional avoidance perpetuates emotional distress [15] and sleep disturbance [63]. However, reducing time in bed can have considerable negative consequences, particularly in young people who are prone to insufficient sleep [10, 60]. Instead, focussing interventions on developing adaptive emotional coping strategies might be beneficial.

Anxiety and bedtime variability

The associations between BPD features, anxiety and bedtime variability were consistent with broader literature demonstrating a relationship between bedtime variability and anxiety in young people, due to day-to-day fluctuations in anxiety and arousal levels [7, 25]. Sleep timing consistency is as important as sleep duration for psychological well-being [25]. Anxiety might therefore be a beneficial treatment target to improve bedtime consistency in young people with BPD features. Again, longitudinal data will help to clarify these cross-sectional observations.

Strengths

This was the first study to utilise objective sleep measures in the exploration of mechanisms underlying sleep disturbance in young people with BPD features. The inclusion of a youth-specific, clinical sample allowed the findings to be representative of a ‘real world’ population of help-seeking young people with BPD features encountered in clinical settings. Further, the investigation of depression, anxiety and stress alongside emotional dysregulation was novel. The use of parallel multiple mediation techniques allowed a direct comparison of potential mediating factors [36]. The findings exposed the importance of considering anxiety in the field of sleep and BPD, which has been largely neglected in prior research [79].

Limitations

The current study was limited by its small scale longitudinal nature . While the 10 day-study provided valid sleep parameters and insights into intra-individual sleep variability, it was limited in assessing the temporal ordering or causal relationships between BPD features, emotion dysregulation, depression, anxiety, stress, and sleep disturbance in young people. Additionally, this study was limited in testing mediation [52]. Similar to Grove et al. [29], results from this study should be interpreted as being consistent with a mediational role in the relationship between BPD features and sleep disturbance, rather than mediation per se. Further, sleep disturbance and emotion dysregulation are bidirectionally related [27, 61], yet relationships explored in the current study were unidirectional. Future research should explore the bidirectional and longitudinal associations between BPD features, emotion dysregulation, depression, anxiety, stress, and sleep disturbance in young people. Additionally, examining the roles of BPD severity and medication use were beyond the scope of the current study and should be explored in future.

While actigraphy is a well-accepted and highly utilised method of objective sleep measurement, it cannot provide the same level of detailed sleep analysis as polysomnography and is limited by relatively poor specificity [1]. As such, objective sleep disturbances (eg. nighttime awakenings) might have been underestimated in the current study. Future research should examine the mechanisms underlying polysomnography-assessed sleep parameters in young people with BPD features.

Conclusion

The current findings suggest that targeting impulse control, emotion regulation strategies and anxiety, through reducing external locus of control and pre-sleep arousal, might be beneficial in improving subjective sleep disturbance in young people with BPD features. Further, addressing emotional awareness and anxiety might be beneficial in further normalising objective sleep patterns in this population. These findings provide helpful guidance for future research hypotheses and the development of targeted sleep-improvement strategies that might improve current interventions for young people with BPD features.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Acebo C. Actigraphy. In: Teofilo L, editor. Sleep: a comprehensive handbook. Wiley; 2005. p. 1035–8.

  2. Alvaro PK, Roberts RM, Harris JK. A systematic review assessing bidirectionality between sleep disturbances, anxiety, and depression. Sleep. 2013;36(7):1059–68. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.2810.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; 2013. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.

  4. Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Broocks A, Riemann D, Hohagen F. Test–retest reliability and validity of the Pittsburgh sleep quality index in primary insomnia. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53(3):737–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00330-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bauducco SV, Salihovic S, Boersma K. Bidirectional associations between adolescents’ sleep problems and impulsive behavior over time. Sleep Med X. 2019;1:100009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Baum KT, Desai A, Field J, Miller LE, Rausch J, Beebe DW. Sleep restriction worsens mood and emotion regulation in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(2):180–90. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Biggs SN, Lushington K, van den Heuvel CJ, Martin AJ, Kennedy JD. Inconsistent sleep schedules and daytime behavioral difficulties in school-aged children. Sleep Med. 2011;12(8):780–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2011.03.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Carney CE, Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, Krystal AD, Lichstein KL, et al. The consensus sleep diary: standardizing prospective sleep self-monitoring. Sleep. 2012;35(2):287–302. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.1642.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Carskadon MA. Adolescent sleep patterns: biological, social, and psychological influences. Cambridge University Press; 2002.

  11. Chanen AM, Betts JK, Jackson H, Cotton SM, Gleeson J, Davey CG, et al. A comparison of adolescent versus young adult outpatients with first-presentation borderline personality disorder: findings from the MOBY randomized controlled trial. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2021;67(1):706743721992677–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/0706743721992677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Chanen AM, Jovev M, Jackson HJ. Adaptive functioning and psychiatric symptoms in adolescents with borderline personality disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2007;68(2):297–306. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v68n0217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Chanen AM, McCutcheon LK, Germano D, Nistico H, Jackson HJ, Mcgorry PD. The HYPE clinic: an early intervention service for borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatr Pract. 2009;15(3):163–72. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.pra.0000351876.51098.f0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Chanen AM, Nicol K, Betts JK, Bond GR, Mihalopoulos C, Jackson HJ, et al. INdividual vocational and educational support trial (INVEST) for young people with borderline personality disorder: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials. 2020;21(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-020-04471-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Chapman AL, Dixon-Gordon KL, Walters KN. Experiential avoidance and emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder. J Ration Emot Cogn Behav Ther. 2011;29(1):35–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10942-011-0124-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Chapman AL, Leung DW, Lynch TR. Impulsivity and emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. J Personal Disord. 2008;22(2):148–64. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2008.22.2.148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Chiu VW, Ree M, Janca A, Waters F. Sleep in schizophrenia: exploring subjective experiences of sleep problems, and implications for treatment. Psychiatry Q. 2016;87(4):633–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-015-9415-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Chóliz M. Mobile-phone addiction in adolescence: the test of mobile phone dependence (TMD). Progress in health sciences. 2012;2(1):33–44.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Conklin CZ, Bradley R, Westen D. Affect regulation in borderline personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2006;194(2):69–77. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.nmd.0000198138.41709.4f.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Deckert M, Schmoeger M, Auff E, Willinger U. Subjective emotional arousal: an explorative study on the role of gender, age, intensity, emotion regulation difficulties, depression and anxiety symptoms, and meta-emotion. Psychol Res. 2020;84(7):1857–76. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01197-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Fairholme CP, Nosen EL, Nillni YI, Schumacher JA, Tull MT, Coffey SF. Sleep disturbance and emotion dysregulation as transdiagnostic processes in a comorbid sample. Behav Res Ther. 2013;51(9):540–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.05.014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. First M, Williams J, Karg R, Spitzer R. Structured clinical interview for DSM-5—research version (SCID-5 for DSM-5, research version; SCID-5-RV). American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2015.

  23. First M, Williams JB, Benjamin LS, Spitzer RL. Structured clinical interview for DSM-5 personality disorders: SCID-5-PD. American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2016.

  24. Fitzpatrick S, Maich K, Carney C, Kuo J. Identifying specific insomnia components in borderline personality disorder and their influence on emotion dysregulation. Personal Disord Theory Res Treat. 2020;11(6):440–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Fuligni AJ, Hardway C. Daily variation in adolescents' sleep, activities, and psychological well-being. J Res Adolesc. 2006;16(3):353–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2006.00498.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Glenn CR, Klonsky ED. Emotion dysregulation as a core feature of borderline personality disorder. J Personal Disord. 2009;23(1):20–8. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.1.20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Goldschmied JR. How sleep shapes emotion regulation. In: sleep, personality, and social behavior. Springer; 2019. p. 83–97.

  28. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26(1):41–54. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Grove JL, Smith TW, Crowell SE, Ellis JH. Preliminary evidence for emotion dysregulation as a mechanism underlying poor sleep quality in borderline personality disorder. J Personal Disord. 2017;31(4):542–52. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2016_30_261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Hafizi S. Sleep and borderline personality disorder: a review. Asian J Psychiatr. 2013;6(6):452–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2013.06.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hantsoo L, Khou CS, White CN, Ong JC. Gender and cognitive–emotional factors as predictors of pre-sleep arousal and trait hyperarousal in insomnia. J Psychosom Res. 2013;74(4):283–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.01.014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Harbard E, Allen NB, Trinder J, Bei B. What's keeping teenagers up? Prebedtime behaviors and actigraphy-assessed sleep over school and vacation. J Adolesc Health. 2016;58(4):426–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.12.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Harvey AG. A cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40(8):869–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00061-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Harvey AG, Murray G, Chandler RA, Soehner A. Sleep disturbance as transdiagnostic: consideration of neurobiological mechanisms. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31(2):225–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Harvey C-J, Gehrman P, Espie CA. Who is predisposed to insomnia: a review of familial aggregation, stress-reactivity, personality and coping style. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;18(3):237–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2013.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. Guilford publications; 2017.

  37. Hope NH, Wakefield MA, Northey L, Chapman AL. The association between locus of control, emotion regulation and borderline personality disorder features. Personal Ment Health. 2018;12(3):241–51. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Huỳnh C, Guilé J-M, Breton J-J, Godbout R. Sleep-wake patterns of adolescents with borderline personality disorder and bipolar disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;47(2):202–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-015-0557-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Jacobson NS, Martell CR, Dimidjian S. Behavioral activation treatment for depression: returning to contextual roots. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2001;8(3):255–70. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.8.3.255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Jenkins CA, Tiley LCF, Lay I, Hartmann JA, Chan JKM, Nicholas CL. (2021c). Comparing GENEActiv against Actiwatch-2 over seven nights using a common sleep scoring algorithm and device-specific wake thresholds. Behav Sleep Med. 2021c:1–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2021.1924175.

  41. Jenkins CA, Thompson KN, Chanen AM, Hartmann JA, Nicol K, Nicholas CL. Subjective and objective sleep in young people with borderline personality disorder features. J Sleep Res. 2021a:e13463. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13463.

  42. Jenkins CA, Thompson KN, Nicholas CL, Chanen AM. Sleep in Young people with features of borderline personality disorder: a scoping review. J Personal Disord. 2021b;36(1):1–19. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2021_35_525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991;14(6):540–5. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/14.6.540.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1992;15(4):376–81. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/15.4.376.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Joormann J, Gotlib IH. Emotion regulation in depression: relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognit Emot. 2010;24(2):281–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903407948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Kaplan KA, Harvey AG. Hypersomnia across mood disorders: a review and synthesis. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13(4):275–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2008.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Karukivi M, Vahlberg T, Horjamo K, Nevalainen M, Korkeila J. Clinical importance of personality difficulties: diagnostically sub-threshold personality disorders. BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17(1):16. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1200-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Kuo JR, Linehan MM. Disentangling emotion processes in borderline personality disorder: physiological and self-reported assessment of biological vulnerability, baseline intensity, and reactivity to emotionally evocative stimuli. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009;118(3):531–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016392.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF. Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales. 2nd ed; 1995. Psychology Foundation

    Google Scholar 

  50. Maich KH, Lachowski AM, Carney CE. Psychometric properties of the consensus sleep diary in those with insomnia disorder. Behav Sleep Med. 2018;16(2):117–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2016.1173556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Maislin G, Pack AI, Kribbs NB, Smith PL, Schwartz AR, Kline LR, et al. A survey screen for prediction of apnea. Sleep. 1995;18(3):158–66. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/18.3.158.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Maxwell SE, Cole DA. Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychol Methods. 2007;12(1):23–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Moreira H, Gouveia MJ, Canavarro MC. A bifactor analysis of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale - short form (DERS-SF) in a sample of adolescents and adults. Curr Psychol. 2020:1–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00602-5.

  54. Morin CM, Barlow DH. Insomnia: psychological assessment and management. Guilford Press; 1993.

  55. Morin CM, Belleville G, Bélanger L, Ivers H. The insomnia severity index: psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep. 2011;34(5):601–8. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/34.5.601.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Mullin BC, Harvey AG, Hinshaw SP. A preliminary study of sleep in adolescents with bipolar disorder, ADHD, and non-patient controls. Bipolar Disord. 2011;13(4):425–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00933.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Nutt D, Wilson S, Paterson L. Sleep disorders as core symptoms of depression. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2008;10(3):329–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Oakley N. Validation with polysomnography of the Sleepwatch sleep/wake scoring algorithm used by the Actiwatch activity monitoring system. Bend: Mini Mitter, Cambridge Neurotechnology; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Oltmanns JR, Oltmanns TF. Borderline personality pathology, polysomnography, and self-reported sleep problems: a review. Curr Sleep Med Rep. 2015;1(2):141–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40675-015-0011-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Owens JA. Insufficient sleep in adolescents and young adults: an update on causes and consequences. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):e921–32. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-1696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Palagini L, Moretto U, Dell'Osso L, Carney C. Sleep-related cognitive processes, arousal, and emotion dysregulation in insomnia disorder: the role of insomnia-specific rumination. Sleep Med. 2017;30:97–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2016.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Palmer CA, Alfano CA. Sleep and emotion regulation: an organizing, integrative review. Sleep Med Rev. 2017;31:6–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2015.12.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Palmer CA, Oosterhoff B, Bower JL, Kaplow JB, Alfano CA. Associations among adolescent sleep problems, emotion regulation, and affective disorders: findings from a nationally representative sample. J Psychiatr Res. 2018;96:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.09.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Papadimitriou GN, Linkowski P. Sleep disturbance in anxiety disorders. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2005;17(4):229–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540260500104524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Salters-Pedneault K, Roemer L, Tull MT, Rucker L, Mennin DS. Evidence of broad deficits in emotion regulation associated with chronic worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Cogn Ther Res. 2006;30(4):469–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9055-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Schäfer JÖ, Naumann E, Holmes EA, Tuschen-Caffier B, Samson AC. Emotion regulation strategies in depressive and anxiety symptoms in youth: a meta-analytic review. J Youth Adolesc. 2017;46(2):261–76. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-016-0585-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Schmidt RE, Harvey AG, Van der Linden M. Cognitive and affective control in insomnia. Front Psychol. 2011;2:349. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Selby EA, Ribeiro JD, Joiner TE Jr. What dreams may come: emotional cascades and nightmares in borderline personality disorder. Dreaming. 2013;23(2):126–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Semiz UB, Basoglu C, Ebrinc S, Cetin M. Nightmare disorder, dream anxiety, and subjective sleep quality in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008;62(1):48–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01789.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Shahid A, Wilkinson K, Marcu S, Shapiro CM. Sleep locus of control scale (SLOC). In: STOP, THAT and one hundred other sleep scales. Springer; 2011. p. 335–9.

  71. Tang NK, Harvey AG. Effects of cognitive arousal and physiological arousal on sleep perception. Sleep. 2004;27(1):69–78. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/27.1.69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Thompson KN, Jackson H, Cavelti M, Betts J, McCutcheon L, Jovev M, et al. The clinical significance of subthreshold borderline personality disorder features in outpatient youth. J Personal Disord. 2019;33(1):71–81. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2018_32_330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Van Veen MM, Karsten J, Lancel M. Poor sleep and its relation to impulsivity in patients with antisocial or borderline personality disorders. Behav Med. 2017;43(3):218–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/08964289.2017.1313719.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Vincent N, Walsh K, Lewycky S. Sleep locus of control and computerized cognitive-behavioral therapy (cCBT). Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(8):779–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Wall K, Vanwoerden S, Penner F, Patriquin M, Alfano CA, Sharp C. Adolescent Sleep Disturbance, Emotion Regulation and Borderline Features in an Inpatient Setting. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2020:1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2020.1772081.

  76. Weiss NH, Tull MT, Anestis MD, Gratz KL. The relative and unique contributions of emotion dysregulation and impulsivity to posttraumatic stress disorder among substance dependent inpatients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;128(1–2):45–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.07.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Williams DP, Cash C, Rankin C, Bernardi A, Koenig J, Thayer JF. Resting heart rate variability predicts self-reported difficulties in emotion regulation: a focus on different facets of emotion regulation. Front Psychol. 2015;6:261. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00261.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Winsper C, Lereya ST, Marwaha S, Thompson A, Eyden J, Singh SP. The aetiological and psychopathological validity of borderline personality disorder in youth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;44:13–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.12.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Winsper C, Tang NKY, Marwaha S, Lereya ST, Gibbs M, Thompson A, et al. The sleep phenotype of borderline personality disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;73:48–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Zimmerman M, Chelminski I, Young D, Dalrymple K, Martinez J. Is dimensional scoring of borderline personality disorder important only for subthreshold levels of severity? J Personal Disord. 2013;27(2):244–51. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2013.27.2.244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the invaluable contributions made by young people and their families who have attended Orygen. The authors would also like to acknowledge the support of our clinical and research colleagues at Orygen.

Funding

This work was supported by a NorthWestern Mental Health Seeding Grant. The funding body was not involved in the design of the study, or collection, analysis and interpretation of data or in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Each author (CJ, KT, CN, JH and AC) made a significant contribution to study conceptualisation, planning and interpretation of findings. CJ collected and analyzed all data and drafted the manuscript. Authors KT, CN, JH and AC revised the initial drafts. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrew M. Chanen.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The current study adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by Melbourne Health Human Research Ethics Committee (#2017.153). Informed consent was obtained from all participants and from a parent or guardian for participants aged under 18 years.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

There are no known conflicts of interest to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

Indirect role of emotion regulation difficulties, depression, anxiety and stress in the relationship between group membership and chronotype (an individual’s natural inclination towards morning or evening preference for activity).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jenkins, C.A., Thompson, K.N., Nicholas, C.L. et al. Potential mechanisms underlying sleep disturbance in young people with borderline personality disorder features: an exploratory study. bord personal disord emot dysregul 9, 10 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-022-00180-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-022-00180-2

Keywords