Skip to main content

Irony detection in patients with borderline personality disorder: an experimental study examining schizotypal traits, response biases and empathy

Abstract

Background

In verbal irony we often convey meanings that oppose the literal words. To look behind these words, we need to integrate perspectives of ourselves, others, and their beliefs about us. Although patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD) experience problems in social cognition and schizotypal symptoms, research on irony comprehension mainly focused on the schizophrenic spectrum. Accounting for possible negative biases in BPD, the current study examined the detection of praising and critical irony in a text messaging interface.

Methods

The cross-sectional study included 30 patients and 30 matched controls, who completed measures of cognitive and affective empathy (Interpersonal Reactivity Index, IRI), schizotypal (Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ), and borderline symptoms (Borderline Symptom List; BSL-23) and the irony detection task. The irony task contained critical and praising remarks embedded in text messages. Asking for literality (ironic vs. literal) and intention ratings (critical to praising) of the stimuli, it allowed to analyze the sensitivity of literality detection as well as implicit and explicit response biases in a signal detection framework.

Results

Borderline symptoms explained lower sensitivity for the detection of literal and ironic statements across groups. Whereas HC showed a negativity bias when implicitly asked about the literalness of the statement, patients with BPD perceived praising utterances as less praising when explicitly asked about their perceived intention. Neither empathy nor schizotypy explained outcomes beyond borderline symptoms.

Conclusions

This was the first study to show lower detection of verbal irony in patients with BPD. While patients were less biased when asked about the literality of a statement, they perceived praising remarks as less positive on explicit measurements. The results highlight the importance of congruent, transparent communication in promoting epistemic trust in individuals with BPD.

Introduction

The psychopathology of borderline personality disorder (BPD) manifests in social interaction. In line with this, research on BPD has focused increasingly on the inferences people draw from these interactions, so-called social cognition [13]. One of the numerous concepts within the domain of social cognition is mentalization [4, 5]. It comprises the implicit and explicit understanding of oneself and others [6] and is developed in early social interactions throughout childhood [6]. Mentalization-based theory proposes an errant development of mentalization contributes to etiology of BPD [6, 7]. In a supportive environment, the caregiver shows the child that they are seen as an intentional being by empathetically mirroring the child’s expressed state of mind (e.g., crying). This helps the child internalize a coherent representation of self and others [6]. Additionally, caregivers provide ostensive cues (e.g., turn-taking or appropriate eye contact) to show that they are communicating socially relevant information [8]. It fosters epistemic trust, which is the general assumption that the information we receive from others is accurate, reliable and personally relevant. Epistemic trust assures us that we are not being intentionally misinformed and allows us to integrate information in our knowledge about the world [811]. A history of maltreatment and neglect may facilitate mistrust around communication in patients with BPD, which can make it harder to believe others [10, 1112]. That is, individuals with BPD experience childhood adversity 13 times more than non-clinical individuals, especially emotional abuse and neglect [13]. With an abusive parent, integrating given information can be dangerous [12] and those with BPD may overinterpret hostile motives when there are none. This form of "hypermentalization" [1422] preserves and prolongs interpersonal conflicts [11].

To master the complexity of communication, we not only have to trust the information given to us, but also distrust it from time to time. A prime example is verbal irony, in which the vocal pitch or incongruent context suggest that the speaker intends the opposite of the literal words [23]. Impairments in the comprehension of irony has been mostly demonstrated for autism [24] and in the context of concretism in schizophrenia [2530]. Being equipped with higher mistrust in the first place, it is likely that patients with BPD, too, may have difficulties to decide which information to trust in irony. In line with this and BPD’s eponymous description of the ‘border’ between psychosis and neurosis [31], individuals with BPD share cognitive biases with schizophrenia [32], show schizotypal traits [33], and/or psychotic symptoms [3438]. Such a transdiagnostic symptomatology challenges the differential diagnostic specificity of nonliteral language deficits. Notably, in personality disorders (PD), BPD and schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) are known to co-occur [33, 39]. Hence, with regard to the dimensional alternative model for personality disorders (AMPD), which has been increasingly applied since the DSM V, the question arises as to which PD pathology is responsible for the ironic misinterpretation in previous studies of schizotypy [28, 40].

In irony, there are two causes of misinterpretation: not being able to detect the intention of the speaker, and being able to, but opting for the literal meaning regardless. The first cause is closely related to mentalization. Irony requires recognizing an intention hidden behind literal words. For this reason, it has been studied mainly in research on social cognition [4143] and is used as its direct measure in video-based tasks [14, 44]. One of these tasks, the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC; [44]) has been widely applied in BPD [18, 4547]. The MASC does not specifically examine irony, but uses ironic remarks among other scenarios as a measure of social cognition. Németh et al. [1] showed that in these multimodal tasks, individuals with BPD’s social cognition impairments are most pronounced [1, 48, 49]. Their response formats offer different interpretations of social situations [44], so selecting the right one requires the subject to explicitly compare different mental states [1]. In these tasks, participants with BPD demonstrate reasoning about mental states, but tend to overinterpret social cues [1422]. By contrast, they show no impairments in nonverbal paradigms such as the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task [50] which only requires to identify an emotion based on pictures of the eye region [1]. The authors concluded that the mere detection of emotions seems to be preserved in BPD. Instead, difficulties arise when multiple perspectives need to be explicitly compared. Multiple perspectives, however, are an inevitable part of irony [41, 51, 52]. And understanding irony requires a flexible shift between them - shifts that seems to be harder for individuals with BPD [5355].

Yet, even the full capacity to compare mental states does not necessarily guarantee that a statement will be perceived as ironic. Irony explicitly leaves the intention of the speaker ambiguous and along that room for interpretation. Individuals who tend to perceive others as dishonest may decide to ignore irony, irrespective of their ability to infer mental states. Addressing this distinction in schizophrenia, Parola et al. [27] analyzed both sensitivity (the detection of a communicative intention) and response bias (the tendency to favor a specific response) during indirect speech comprehension. Individuals with schizophrenia had equal difficulty detecting ironic, deceitful, and sincere phrasings, but tended to perceive ironic utterances more deceitful than healthy controls. Negative attribution styles are common in BPD as well [2]; many individuals with BPD tend to interpret others’ behavior as aggressive and hostile [56] and neutral faces as less trustworthy [57]. This places patients in a vicious cycle of reliving traumatic relationships [12, 58]. Therapists are often encouraged to use clear, unambiguous communication to avoid unintentionally reinforcing the threat perceived by their patients [59]. This is especially true as negative biases in BPD tend to develop specifically in the face of ambiguous stimuli [3, 49, 6062].

Using irony as a prime example of ambiguous language allows both pragmatic inference and attributional bias to be examined within one linguistic phenomenon. Most studies on irony comprehension, however, focus solely on ironic criticism or sarcasm [44], thereby confounding irony with an a priori negative bias. Analyzing both praising and critical irony bypasses positive testing and allows interpretation errors to be analyzed without overtly asking for them. For example, Kieckhäfer et al. [63] examined how the detection of ironic and literal praising and critical relate to borderline and schizotypal traits in healthy adults. In their study, both traits were associated with lower detection accuracy, though each set of traits had differing error patterns. In line with Parola et al.’s findings in schizophrenia [27], individuals with higher schizotypy interpreted the stimuli more mocking: They indicated literal praise as ironic critique and ironic praise as literal critique. In contrast, individuals with high borderline traits only made errors identifying ironic remarks and this was regardless of the intention.

We applied Kieckhaefer et al.’s [63] paradigm, for the first time, on participants diagnosed with BPD. We compared the detection of literality (ironic vs. literal) and implicit response biases within a signal detection theory (SDT) framework, as well as explicit ratings of the perceived intention (critical to praising) with healthy controls (HC). In accordance with findings on healthy adults with borderline symptoms [63], we hypothesized that participants with BPD would have more difficulty differentiating ironic and literal utterances. We further assumed that negativity biases would emerge in a more pronounced BPD symptomatology. Thus, in contrast to Kieckhäfer et al.’s results [63], we expected participants with BPD to interpret ironic praise and literal criticism literally, and ironic criticism and literal praise ironically. In line with this, we predicted BPD participants would rate critical remarks as more critical and praising remarks as less praising. To clarify the specific contributions of borderline and schizotypal symptoms on irony and to consider a more dimensional assessment of PD, we examined the relationship between these characteristics and irony comprehension across groups. Last, we included possible mentalizing capacities related to irony comprehension in BPD via affective and cognitive empathy.

Methods

30 participants with BPD were recruited from the University Hospital of Tuebingen, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Germany. The ward was specialized on dialectical behavioral therapy (DBT, [64]). General exclusion criteria were acute or anamnestic substance abuse or dependence, bipolar disorder, psychotic disorders, severe episodes of major depression, and neurological diseases. Inclusion criteria involved normal or corrected-to-normal vision, age between 18–55, native German speakers, and a clinical diagnosis of BPD for the patient group. A trained clinician assessed the DSM-IV criteria according to Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV II (SCID II) [65] and comorbidities according to SCID I [66]. Except for 7 individuals, patients exhibited comorbid diagnoses, which included depressive disorders (n = 13), post traumatic stress disorder (n = 11), substance use but abstinent for at least 2 months (n = 1) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (n = 1). However, none of them fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for other personality disorders according to the traditional DSM-IV model. The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Tuebingen and carried out according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent and received monetary compensation.

A group of 30 healthy controls (HC), was matched for age, verbal intelligence according to the multiple-choice vocabulary test (MWT, [67]), gender, and educational level. Both groups filled out the short version of the Borderline Symptom List (BSL-23, [68]) and the German version of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ, [69, 70]). For the evaluation of cognitive and affective empathy, the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI, [71]) was used as a German short version [72]. The IRI is a self-report instrument comprising two cognitive subscales (perspective taking, fantasy) and two affective subscales (empathic concern, personal distress).

After consenting to participate, demographics were assessed in paper-pencil format. Then, participants completed the irony paradigm and self-report instruments on a computer in a quiet, distraction-free room. The stimuli were the same as in Kieckhäfer et al. [63]; test construction and development are explained in detail there. Each trial consisted of a videotaped context story introducing a character in a café and subsequent message exchanges. According to the narrative, participants saw text messages containing a context sentence and a reaction to that message by the protagonist of the video (see Additional file 1). The message was either ironic praise (IP), ironic criticism (IC), literal praise (LP), or literal criticism (LC). In ironic stimuli, the intended meaning opposed the literal meaning. For example, IP had a praising intention by way of critique (“I went running today” “You are so lazy.”). Videos varied in the degree of proximal perspective, and were either addressed directly by the protagonist (2nd person) or observed by the protagonist talking to a neutral other (3rd person). The protagonist’s answers were to be scored on their literality (ironic vs. literal) in a binary response format, and their perceived intention (criticism vs. praise) on a five-point Likert scale (see Additional file 2). Each trial comprised five items per condition (20 items total). Participants completed two test versions, with one perspective each. Summation of correct identified items lead to a total maximum score of 10 correct responses per condition (IP, IC, LP, LC) for both test versions.

We applied SDT to quantify sensitivity (d’) and response biases (β). As in SDT designs, the irony task required a binary label of literality (literal vs. ironic), which could be compared to the presence or absence of a signal (irony present vs. irony absent), resulting in four logical outcomes (Table 1): hit (choosing ironic in an ironic stimulus), false alarm (choosing ironic in a literal stimulus), miss (choosing literal in an ironic stimulus), and correct rejection (choosing literal in a literal stimulus). Each category was assigned a likelihood ratio. For instance, the hit rate represents the proportion of ironic stimuli to which the participant responded “ironic”, and false alarm rate denotes the proportion of literal trials to which the participant responded “ironic”. Unlike the mere number of correct responses, SDT’s measure of sensitivity reflects the probability of identifying the intention of the stimulus while avoiding false alarms, and corresponds to the Z-value of the hit rate minus the false-alarm rate.

Table 1 Signal detection theory matrix with possible outcomes for each contrastive pair of stimuli

SDT further accounts for the response bias β: a systematic criterion when a signal is considered as present. It can capture the tendency of an individual to interpret statements either as ironic or literal. An individual who tends to interpret statements as “ironic” shows high hits for ironic (IC and IP), but high false alarms in literal stimuli (LC and LP). An unbiased observer’s β is close to 1. With a tendency to respond “ironic” (liberal criterion), β approaches 0. With the tendency to choose “literal” (conservative criterion), β exceeds 1. d’ and β were computed with the R package psycho. The binary answer format (ironic vs. literal) and definition of irony as the opposite of the literal meaning resulted in two corresponding conditions (IP vs. LC; IC vs. LP). Specifically, in an IC stimulus (“I am too late.” “You are so reliable.”), the detection of the correct literality (i.e., “ironic”) requires detecting the critical intention, despite the literal praise. The same holds true for IP and LC for a praising intention. For each participant, we calculated the hits, false alarms, misses, and correct rejections for both matching pairs.

Then, we applied linear mixed effect models in R with the lme4 package using d’, β, and ratings of perceived intention as respective outcome; group (HC vs. BPD) and intention (praise vs. criticism) as sum-coded fixed effects; age and verbal intelligence as continuous covariates; gender as a categorical covariate; and random effects by participant. Post-hoc tests with adjusted p-values were carried out with Tukey’s test. Based on the stimulus design, misclassifying literality causes perception of the opposite intention (e.g., ironic praise as literal criticism). Thus, perceived intention was estimated by the mean rating of items correctly identified as ironic or literal.

For each model, the impact of borderline symptoms, schizotypal symptoms, and empathy scales was analyzed. Model fits were estimated hierarchically, starting out with the null model, then adding borderline and schizotypal symptoms, and finally IRI subscales, as fixed effects. Models were compared via Likelihood-ratio tests using the anova function.

Similar to previous results [63], the perspective of the speaker had no effect on detection performance in a preceding repeated measure Analysis of Variance (rmANOVA, see supplementary Table 1). Thus, the conditions were not included in analysis.

Results

Groups did not differ significantly in age (t (58) = − .812, p = .420), gender (Z = .417, p = .519), educational level (Z = −1.736, p = .083), or verbal intelligence (t (58) = −1.062, p = .293). Patients with BPD had significantly more borderline symptoms (t (38.46) = −8.971, p < .001) and personal distress (t (58) = −6.215, p < .001), as well as lower perspective taking (t (58) = 2.871, p = .006) and more schizotypal symptoms (t (58) = −8.662, p < .001). A detailed sample description can be found in Table 2.

Table 2 Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of demographic and psychometric data

Final models are depicted in Table 3. There was no effect of gender, age, or verbal IQ. Despite possible ceiling effects, patients with BPD (M = 1.00, SD = 0.32) showed significantly less sensitivity d' than HC (M = 1.12, SD = 0.22) in differentiating ironic and literal statements (t (58) = 2.184, p = .033), regardless of the intention (see Fig. 1). Above groups, sensitivity was higher for IC vs. LP (M = 1.14, SD = .27) than IP vs. LC (M = .98, SD = .28; t (58) = 4.22, p < .001).

Table 3 Analysis of deviance table (Type II Wald chi-square tests) for the linear mixed effect models with sum-coded contrasts and random intercepts by subject of sensitivity d' (left) and response bias β (right) including borderline symptoms
Fig. 1
figure 1

Sensitivity (dprime d') values for BPD and HC groups

For response bias, there was a significant interaction of group on intention. However, post-hoc pairwise comparisons of group by level of intention indicated group differences in β for both IC vs. LP (t (116) = − 2.313, p = .023) and IP vs. LC (t (116) = 2.321, p = .022). On a descriptive level, β tended to be closer to 1 in BPD (see Table 1 and Fig. 2), indicating that BPD participants were almost unbiased. In contrast, HC showed a lower β in ironic criticism, corresponding with a tendency to interpret an answer as ironic in IC and LP and thus as mocking. The same negativity bias was evident in the other pair, with HC having a higher β in IP vs. LC and a tendency to choose literal. There was a significant effect of gender, with males having higher β than females (t (55) = − 2.027, p = .048).

Borderline symptoms significantly improved model fit for d’ (χ2 (1) = 5.497, p = .019), with a significant effect on d’ diminishing the effect of group. Neither SPQ (χ2 (1) = .187, p = .633), nor IRI subscales (χ2 (4) = 2.302, p = .680) improved model fit. For β, neither BSL (χ2 (1) = 0.011, p = .917), nor SPQ (χ2 (1) = 0.517, p = .472) or IRI scales (χ2 (1) = 0.064, p = .999) improved model fit.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Response bias β for the comparisons of ironic criticism (IC) with literal praise (LP) and ironic praise (IP) with literal criticism (LC). An unbiased β corresponds to 1, a β approaching zero a tendency to choose ironic, a β increasing over one a tendency to choose literal as response

In the last step we analyzed the ratings of perceived intention (Table 4). There was a significant interaction of group*intention, with clinical participants perceiving praising remarks as less praising (t (172) = 3.480, p < .001), but no difference in the perception of critical remarks (t (172) = −1.133, p = .259). Post-hoc comparisons for the interaction of intention*literal indicated all pairwise comparisons to be significant (all p < .0001), confirming previous findings that ironic remarks were perceived as less praising (IP: M = 2.21, SD = .63 vs. LP: M = 4.61, SD = .33) and less critical (IC: M = 2.21, SD = .54 vs LC: M = 1.87, SD = 46) in both groups. Again, neither BSL (χ2 (1) = 0.472, p = .491), SPQ (χ2 (1) = .740, p = .187), nor IRI scales (χ2 (1) = 5.017, p = .414) improved model fit.

Table 4 Analysis of deviance table (Type II Wald chi-square tests) for the linear mixed model of perceived intention with sum-coded contrasts and random intercepts by subject

Discussion

This was the first study to examine irony comprehension among individuals with BPD. Participants were presented with both ironic and literal text messages varying in praising and critical intention. Within a signal detection framework, we assessed response biases and the ability to discriminate literal from ironic remarks. Biases were distinguished on two levels: implicit tendencies measured in the choice of the literalness of the statement (ironic vs. literal) and explicit ratings of perceived intention (critical to praising).

Participants with BPD exhibited more difficulty differentiating literal from ironic remarks than HC. Yet, group differences did not vary with critical or praising content, showing that it was the literality of the stimulus, not the intention, affecting performance. For both groups, ironic praise was harder to detect than ironic criticism, replicating that ironic criticism is easier to process [73, 74], mostly because it is much more common [7476]. The current results are commensurate with those in social cognition paradigms using sarcasm as a stimulus [15, 22, 46]. For the first time, these impairments have been confirmed with respect to verbal irony. Importantly, borderline symptoms explained reduced sensibility beyond categorical groups, confirming findings among healthy adults with borderline traits in a clinical sample [63] and corroborating dimensional approaches to personality disorders [77].

Other forms of nonliteral language, such as metaphors, have recently been shown to be preserved in BPD [78]. This is of particular importance, as metaphor comprehension is commonly impaired in schizophrenia [79] with whom BPD patients share symptoms [32, 33]. Contrary to other studies [28, 29, 40, 63, 80], schizotypal symptoms did not explain irony detection beyond borderline symptoms, although patients scored high on both. Our results support the idea that different forms of nonliteral language are subject to different cognitive processes [27, 81, 82]. And it implies that different expressions of psychopathologies may have their own causes of miscomprehension. For example, the cause of schizophrenic concretism has traditionally been understood as a difficulty with abstraction [83, 84]. In that sense, both metaphor and irony require an abstraction from the literal words, but irony further demands to integrate multiple mental states [8, 85, 86]. It is yet to explore whether specific sets of personality traits have their own processes hindering the comprehension of nonliteral language. To analyze this, there is a strong need to include assessments of abstraction (e.g., Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, [87]) and ecologically valid social cognition paradigms (e.g., The Awareness of Social Inference Test, [88]) as well as different forms of nonliteral language in future research.

Ironists do not intend to deceive but seek duplicitous understanding. As such, irony proves particularly challenging for mentalizing: It requires the listener to identify the other’s and own perspective, their relation and context. In our study, errors indicated that in some instances patients decided to stick to the literal meaning, even when an incongruence between context and target sentence suggested otherwise. Reduced mentalization may make it more difficult for individuals with BPD to decide which of these two meanings the speaker wants them to believe [10]. As a solution, they may adhere to one of them [16, 89] and choose a context-inappropriate interpretation. Indeed, shifts in the representation of the self and of others have long been deemed problematic in BPD [31]. They constitute the main personality psychopathology captured in Criterion A of the DSM-5 [90] and are largely represented by borderline symptoms [91]. Empirically, patients with BPD experience difficulties alternating between egocentric and altercentric perspectives with face-morphing tasks [54] and show overlapping self-other boundaries on a bodily and cognitive level [53, 54]. Accordingly, in our study, patient’s personal distress in response to others’ emotions was higher and cognitive perspective-taking lower than those of HC, replicating previous findings on self-reported empathy scales in BPD [9294]. However, both were unrelated to outcomes in our study of irony. Future studies should include more complex social cognition paradigms that may be more commensurate with metacognitive processes than with self-ratings [48], and speech varying in self-other representation (e.g., deceit and faux pas). Instead of categorical groups, it will be essential to dimensionally assess impairments in self and interpersonal functioning (criterion A, [90]); e.g., via the Levels of Personality Functioning Scale (LPFS, [95]), as well as maladaptive personality variants (criterion B, [96, 97]).

Contrary to our expectations, HC (and not BPD) tended to interpret stimuli critically when deciding whether a remark was meant literally or ironically. Interpreting literal praise ironically HC ascribed negative intent to literal praise (“I have an A in my test” “You are clearly not smart”), while ironic criticism was seen as literal criticism. The same negativity bias was evident in ironic praise and literal criticism: HC tended to interpret these statements literally, considering ironic praise as literal critique and literal critique as such. What could be the reason for this? In our everyday conversations, ironic remarks usually express a critical attitude [74, 98, 99]. So, when asked to look for irony, a negative bias is a strategy that promises the most success. In terms of our cultural knowledge and experience, HCs decisions about whether or not to trust the literal remarks were therefore appropriate. Patients with BPD who grew up in an unreliable [6, 10, 11] communicative environment may not have developed such stable expectations about when interlocutors use what communicative intent [11]. And in a state of epistemic mistrust, a repeated experience such as “irony is typically negative” may not be internalized and generalized to other social contexts [11]. Instead, the trustworthiness of each new statement is assessed de novo. This can create uncertainty about what to expect and prevent efficient calibration of epistemic confidence, especially in ambiguous situations. Without stable priors, choosing whether to trust the negative (positive) context or the positive (negative) comment may thus be more random.

When it comes to explicit ratings of a critical or praising intent, the results are consistent with previous research on negativity biases. Both groups tended to rate ironic utterances as less praising and critical than literal ones, confirming the well-known perception of irony as “tinged” with the literal meaning [100, 101]. In contrast to implicit biases, patients with BPD perceived praising remarks as less praising than HC. So far, only a limited number of studies have investigated the effect of positive social stimuli in BPD [49]. Our findings are in line with BPD participants’ fear of positive appraisal [102], negative ratings of appreciating video-clips [103] or self-referential information [104] and approach-avoidance behavior [105, 106]. Muting the positive experience of praise has major implications, since positive feedback is a crucial part of the therapeutic process [107] and of positive interactions with others. Yet, contrary to other studies [103] and patients with BPD’s heightened rejection sensitivity [108] clinical participants did not differ to HC in perception of critical remarks.

The current study differs on multiple dimensions to tasks that often report a negativity bias [49, 109, 110]. First, it only included verbal stimuli. Negativity biases in BPD have mostly been found in non-verbal tasks such as facial emotion recognition [3, 49, 111, 112], especially in combination with other modalities [113, 114]. Second, we only assessed criticism and praise. Most biases in BPD regard anger and disgust [3, 111] or neutral stimuli [3, 61, 115]. Criticism only expresses dissatisfaction and less intense than anger. Further, irony is impossible to be neutral, as its principal function is to tacitly convey an opinion of the ironized content [52]. Third, implicit biases were assessed by asking participants to indicate the literalness of the stimulus (ironic vs. literal). In addition to explicit ratings of perceived intention, this allowed for a covert assessment of affective biases. In most emotion recognition paradigms, participants are asked directly about the emotion being displayed, which makes emotion as such the focus of attention and activates associated expectations about other people’s affective states. It may be that negativity biases are more pronounced when explicitly asked about the valence rather than the literalness of a statement.

We are aware of several limitations. First, they concern the generalizability of our sample. The patient sample had a high verbal IQ and educational background, which may be less prevalent in BPD among the general population [116, 117]. Patients were recruited in a specialized ward for DBT [118], which trains the differentiation between self and other and emotion regulation. On the one hand, this may have even minimized the group differences in irony detection. On the other hand, active practice of emotion regulation skills may have contributed to the unbiased response pattern in BPD compared to HC. This is further supported by the fact that borderline symptoms showed no association with response bias across groups. Despite inclusion and exclusion criteria, there was a high comorbidity of traditional Axis I disorders, resulting in heterogeneous psychopathology in the sample. Second, other limitations concern the applied paradigm. The difficulty of the stimuli seemed to be limited, which is why ceiling effect may have attenuated effects. Further, our stimuli did not contain nonverbal language such as prosody, facial expression or body posture. In line with this, the concept of irony transcends verbal irony, such as situational irony, hyperbole or understatement [119]. However, the study focused on the messenger interface which is a major part of current communication. Third, we did not account for experienced abuse or neglect, which is associated with epistemic distrust [10]. Lastly, this study did not include clinical controls, leaving the question of clinical specificity to be explored.

Conclusions

This was the first study to provide evidence for an impaired irony detection in patients with BPD. Borderline symptoms explained this effect, but neither schizotypal traits nor empathy scales were related to outcomes of irony. While the use of ambiguous language is claimed to be restricted in therapeutic contexts with BPD patients [7, 107], the current study shows that this claim cannot be generalized to all forms of nonliteral language. With a preserved metaphor [78], but impaired irony comprehension in BPD, it seems that it is not the ambiguity of being nonliteral, but the ambiguity of the intention that imposes an obstacle for BPD. Just as irony forms a sense of collusion for those who understand [120], a patient’s misunderstanding may leave them with feelings of exclusion. In BPD this may even lead to a rupture in the therapeutic relation. Patients with BPD only showed a negative bias when explicitly asked to rate perceived intention. To them, praising remarks were considered less praising. Therapists and research alike naturally focus on negative social perceptions in BPD, but our results highlight the importance of targeting the diminished beneficial effect of positive feedback as well. Both findings emphasize the relevance of a shared and open discussion of the possible inferences BPD patients may draw from social interactions. But they also have significance for therapists. Together, they emphasize the need for practitioners to make their implicit mental states explicit, as encouraged in MBT [11, 107] and in DBT by specifying the dialectic [64, 118]. Our findings place a strong emphasis on MBT’s claim to encourage practitioners to be especially transparent, self-revealing, and explicit about their thoughts to promote epistemic trust, open the epistemic channel to integrate culturally and personally relevant information, and model the capacity for intentional communication in the patient. But they also stress the vital role of the therapist’s communication between the lines. Explicit mentalization is not a fully abstract process, but inherently interwoven with implicit, bodily intersubjectivity [121, 122]. Aligning words with intention and both with posture, prosody, and facial expression can serve to provide a clear basis for exploration of self and others in language.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets and material in the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Abbreviations

AMPD:

Alternative Model of Personality Disorders

rmANOVA:

Repeated measurements analysis of variance

BPD:

Borderline personality disorder

BSL-23:

Borderline Symptom List – short version

DSM-IV:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

HC:

Healthy control participants

IC:

Ironic criticism

IP:

Ironic praise

IRI:

Interpersonal reactivity index

LC:

Literal praise

LP:

Literal criticism

MASC:

Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition

MWT:

German multiple-choice vocabulary test

PD:

Personality disorder

SCID-II:

Structured Clinical Interview for axis II personality disorders

SCID-I:

Structured Clinical Interview for axis I

SDT:

Signal detection theory

SPD:

Schizotypal personality disorder

SPQ:

Schizotypal personality questionnaire

References

  1. Németh N, Mátrai P, Hegyi P, Czéh B, Czopf L, Hussain A, et al. Theory of mind disturbances in borderline personality disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2018;270:143–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.049.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Roepke S, Vater A, Preißler S, Heekeren HR, Dziobek I. Social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Front Neurosci. 2012;4:182. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00182.

  3. Daros AR, Zakzanis KK, Ruocco AC. Facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med. 2013;43(9):1953–63. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712002607.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Brüne M. “Theory of Mind” in Schizophrenia: a review of the literature. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31(1):21–42. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbi002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Dimopoulou T, Tarazi FI, Tsapakis EM. Clinical and therapeutic role of mentalization in schizophrenia - a review. CNS Spectr. 2017;22(6):450–62. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852916000687.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fonagy P, Bateman A. The development of borderline personality disorder - A mentalizing model. J Personal Disord. 2008;22(1):4–21. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2008.22.1.4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Fonagy P, Bateman AW. Attachment Theory and Mentalization-Oriented Model of Borderline Personality Disorder. In: The American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of personality disorders. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; 2005. p. 187–207.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Sperber D, Wilson D. Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind Lang. 2002;17(1–2):3–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Sperber D, Clément F, Heintz C, Mascaro O, Mercier H, Origgi G, et al. Epistemic vigilance. Mind Lang. 2010;25(4):359–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x.

  10. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E. Epistemic petrification and the restoration of epistemic trust: A new conceptualization of borderline personality disorder and its psychosocial treatment. J Personal Disord. 2015;29(5):575–609. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2015.29.5.575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E, Campbell C. What we have changed our minds about: Part 2. Borderline personality disorder, epistemic trust and the developmental significance of social communication. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2017;4(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-017-0062-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Fonagy P. Attachment and Borderline Personality Disorder. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. 2000;48(4):1129–46. https://doi.org/10.1177/00030651000480040701.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Porter C, Palmier-Claus J, Branitsky A, Mansell W, Warwick H, Varese F. Childhood adversity and borderline personality disorder: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2020;141(1):6–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13118.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Sharp C, Ha C, Carbone C, Kim S, Perry K, Williams L, et al. Hypermentalizing in adolescent inpatients: Treatment effects and association with borderline traits. J Personal Disord. 2013;27(SPL.ISS.1):3–18. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2013.27.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Sharp C, Pane H, Ha C, Venta A, Patel AB, Sturek J, et al. Theory of mind and emotion regulation difficulties in adolescents with borderline traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(6):563–73.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Sharp C, Vanwoerden S. Hypermentalizing in borderline personality disorder: a model and data. J Infant Child Adolesc Psychother. 2015;14(1):33–45. .

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Penner F, McLaren V, Leavitt J, Akca OF, Sharp C. Implicit and explicit mentalizing deficits in adolescent inpatients: specificity and incremental value of borderline pathology. 2020;34:64–83. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2019_33_463.

  18. Vaskinn A, Antonsen BT, Fretland RA, Dziobek I, Sundet K, Wilberg T. Theory of mind in women with borderline personality disorder or schizophrenia: differences in overall ability and error patterns. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1239. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Andreou C, Kelm L, Bierbrodt J, Braun V, Lipp M, Yassari AH, et al. Factors contributing to social cognition impairment in borderline personality disorder and schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2015;229(3):872–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.057.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ortega-Díaz E, García-Campos J, Rico-Gomis JM, Cuesta-Moreno C, Palazón-Bru A, Estañ-Cerezo G, et al. Social cognition and social functioning in people with borderline personality disorder and their first-degree relatives. PeerJ. 2020;8:e10212. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.10212.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Normann-Eide E, Antonsen BT, Kvarstein EH, Pedersen G, Vaskinn A, Wilberg T. Are Impairments in Theory of Mind Specific to Borderline Personality Disorder? J Personal Disord. 2020;34(6):827–41. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2019_33_417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Somma A, Ferrara M, Terrinoni A, Frau C, Ardizzone I, Sharp C, et al. Hypermentalizing as a marker of borderline personality disorder in Italian adolescents: a cross-cultural replication of Sharp and colleagues’ (2011) findings. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2019;6(1):1–4. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-019-0104-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lausberg H. Handbuch der literarischen Rhetorik. 3rd ed. Stuttgart: Steiner; 1960. p. 605–957.

  24. Kalandadze T, Norbury C, Nærland T, Næss KAB. Figurative language comprehension in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analytic review. Autism. 2018;22(2):99–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316668652.

  25. Langdon R, Davies M, Coltheart M. Understanding Minds and Understanding Communicated Meanings in Schizophrenia. Mind Lang. 2002;17(1–2):68–104. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Mo S, Su Y, Chan RCK, Liu J. Comprehension of metaphor and irony in schizophrenia during remission: The role of theory of mind and IQ. Psychiatry Res. 2008;157(1–3):21–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Parola A, Brasso C, Morese R, Rocca P, Bosco FM. Understanding communicative intentions in schizophrenia using an error analysis approach. NPJ Schizophr. 2021;7(1):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41537-021-00142-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Rapp AM, Mutschler DE, Wild B, Erb M, Lengsfeld I, Saur R, et al. Neural correlates of irony comprehension: The role of schizotypal personality traits. Brain Lang. 2010;113(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Rapp AM, Langohr K, Mutschler DE, Klingberg S, Wild B, Erb M. Isn’t it ironic? Neural Correlates of Irony Comprehension in Schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2013;8(9):e74224. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0074224.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Varga E, Simon M, Tényi T, Schnell Z, Hajnal A, Orsi G, et al. Irony comprehension and context processing in schizophrenia during remission - a functional MRI study. Brain Lang. 2013;126(3):231–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Kernberg OF. Borderline-Störungen und pathologischer Narzissmus. In: Literatur der Psychoanalyse. 4th ed. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp; 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Puri P, Kumar D, Muralidharan K, Kishore MT. Individuals with Borderline Personality Disorder manifest cognitive biases implicated in psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2018;267:414–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.06.040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Kwapil TR, Clark HE, Rbeiz KS, Bathery AJ, Kemp KC, Barrantes-Vidal N. Association of positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy with cluster a, borderline, and avoidant personality disorders and traits. Personal Disord. 2022;13(2):182-91. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000488.

  34. Cavelti M, Thompson K, Chanen AM, Kaess M. Psychotic symptoms in borderline personality disorder: developmental aspects. Curr Opin Psychol. 2021;37:26–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. D’Agostino A, Rossi Monti M, Starcevic V. Psychotic symptoms in borderline personality disorder: an update. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2019;32(1):22–6. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Moritz S, Schilling L, Wingenfeld K, Köther U, Wittekind C, Terfehr K, et al. Psychotic-like cognitive biases in borderline personality disorder. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42(3):349–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Slotema CW, Daalman K, Blom JD, Diederen KM, Hoek HW, Sommer IEC. Auditory verbal hallucinations in patients with borderline personality disorder are similar to those in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2012;42(9):1873–8. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712000165.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Slotema CW, Blom JD, Deen M, Niemantsverdriet MBA, van der Gaag M, Hoek HW, et al. Negative beliefs about voices in patients with borderline personality disorder are associated with distress: a plea for cognitive-behavioural therapy? Psychopathology. 2017;50(4):255–61. https://doi.org/10.1159/000477669.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Meisner MW, Lenzenweger MF, Bach B, Vestergaard M, Petersen LS, Haahr UH, et al. Exploring Identity Disturbance and Psychotic Spectrum Symptoms as Predictors of Borderline and Schizotypal Personality Disorders. Psychopathology. 2021;54(4):193–202. https://doi.org/10.1159/000516209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Langdon R, Coltheart M. Recognition of metaphor and irony in young adults: the impact of schizotypal personality traits. Psychiatry Res. 2004;125(1):9–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2003.10.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1998;62(1):89–106. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1889.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Happé FGE. An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24(2):129–54. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172093.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Bosco FM, Gabbatore I. Sincere, deceitful, and ironic communicative acts and the role of the theory of mind in childhood. Front Psychol. 2017;8:21. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00021.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Dziobek I, Fleck S, Kalbe E, Rogers K, Hassenstab J, Brand M, et al. Introducing MASC: A movie for the assessment of social cognition. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36(5):623–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Goueli T, Nasreldin M, Madbouly N, Dziobek I, Farouk M. Social cognition in adolescent females with borderline personality traits. Psychol Psychother. 2020;93(4):739–53. https://doi.org/10.1111/papt.12257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Preißler S, Dziobek I, Ritter K, Heekeren H, Roepke S. Social Cognition in Borderline Personality Disorder: Evidence for Disturbed Recognition of the Emotions, Thoughts, and Intentions of others. Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:182. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00182.

  47. Somma A, Ferrara M, Terrinoni A, Frau C, Ardizzone I, Sharp C, et al. Hypermentalizing as a marker of borderline personality disorder in Italian adolescents: a cross-cultural replication of Sharp and colleagues’ (2011) findings. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2019;6(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-019-0104-5.

  48. Jeung H, Herpertz SC. Impairments of interpersonal functioning. Psychopathology. 2014;47(4):220–34. https://doi.org/10.1159/000357191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Bertsch K, Hillmann K, Herpertz SC. Behavioral and neurobiological correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology. 2018;51(2):76–82. https://doi.org/10.1159/000487363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1997;38(7):813–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Nilsen ES, Glenwright M, Huyder V. Children and adults understand that verbal irony interpretation depends on listener knowledge. J Cogn Dev. 2011;12(3):374–409. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2010.544693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Winner E, Gardner H. Metaphor and irony: Two levels of understanding. In: Ortony A, editor. Metaphor and Thought. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2012. p. 425–44. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139173865.021.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  53. Luyten P, de Meulemeester C, Fonagy P. The Self--Other Distinction in Psychopathology: Recent Developments from a Mentalizing Perspective. In: Gilead M, Ochsner KN, editors. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2021. p. 659–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51890-5_34.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  54. de Meulemeester C, Lowyck B, Luyten P. The role of impairments in self–other distinction in borderline personality disorder: A narrative review of recent evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;127:242–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. De Meulemeester C, Lowyck B, Panagiotopoulou E, Fotopoulou A, Luyten P. Self-other distinction and borderline personality disorder features: Evidence for egocentric and altercentric bias in a self-other facial morphing task. Personal Disord. 2021;12(4):377-88. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000415.

  56. Barnow S, Stopsack M, Grabe HJ, Meinke C, Spitzer C, Kronmüller K, et al. Interpersonal evaluation bias in borderline personality disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2009;47(5):359–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Fertuck EA, Grinband J, Stanley B. Facial Trust Appraisal Negatively Biased in Borderline Personality Disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2013;207(3):195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E, Campbell C. What we have changed our minds about: Part 1. Borderline personality disorder as a limitation of resilience. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2017;4:11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-017-0061-9.

  59. Allen JG, Fonagy P, editors. Handbook of mentalization-based treatment. Chichester: Wiley; 2006.

  60. Catalan A, Gonzalez de Artaza M, Bustamante S, Orgaz P, Osa L, Angosto V, et al. Differences in Facial Emotion Recognition between First Episode Psychosis, Borderline Personality Disorder and Healthy Controls. PLoS One. 2016;11(7):e0160056. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160056.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Domes G, Schulze L, Herpertz SC. Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder-a review of the literature. J Personal Disord. 2009;23(1):6–19. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.1.6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Kaiser D, Jacob GA, van Zutphen L, Siep N, Sprenger A, Tuschen-Caffier B, et al. Biased attention to facial expressions of ambiguous emotions in borderline personality disorder: An eye-tracking study. J Personal Disord. 2019;33(5):671–90. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2019_33_363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Kieckhaefer C, Felsenheimer AK, Rapp AM. A new test for irony detection. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:99. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Heard HL, Linehan MM. Dialectical behavior therapy: an integrative approach to the treatment of borderline personality disorder. J Psychother Integr. 1994;4(1):55–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Fydrich T, Renneberg B, Schmitz B, Wittchen HU. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Wittchen HU, Zaudig M, Fydrich T. SKID. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV Achse I. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Lehrl S, Triebig G, Fischer B. Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence. Acta Neurol Scand. 1995;91(5):335–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1995.tb07018.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Bohus M, Kleindienst N, Limberger MF, Stieglitz RD, Domsalla M, Chapman AL, et al. The short version of the Borderline Symptom List (BSL-23): Development and initial data on psychometric properties. Psychopathology. 2009;42(1):32–9. https://doi.org/10.1159/000173701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Klein C, Andresen B, Jahn T. Psychometric assessment of the schizotypal personality according to DSM-III-R criteria Psychometric properties of an authorized German translation of Raine’s “Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire” (SPQ). Diagnostica. 1997;43(4):347–69.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Raine A. The SPQ: A Scale for the Assessment of Schizotypal Personality Based on DSM-III-R Criteria. Schizophr Bull. 1991;17(4):555–64. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/17.4.555.

  71. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44(1):113–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Paulus C. Der Saarbrücker Persönlichkeitsfragebogen (SPF-IRI) zur Messung von Empathie. 2009; Available from: http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11780/3343

    Google Scholar 

  73. Giora R. On Irony and Negation. Discourse Process. 1995;19(2):239–64. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539509544916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Kreuz RJ, Link KE. Asymmetries in the use of verbal irony. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2002;21(2):127–43+191. https://doi.org/10.1177/02627X02021002002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Clark HH, Gerrig RJ. On the pretense theory of irony. J Exp Psychol. 1984;113(1):121–6. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.113.1.121.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Gibbs R. Irony in Talk Among Friends. Metaphor Symb. 2000;15(1):5–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2000.9678862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Herpertz SC, Huprich SK, Bohus M, Chanen A, Goodman M, Mehlum L, et al. The challenge of transforming the diagnostic system of personality disorders. J Personal Disord. 2017;31(5):577–89. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2017_31_338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Felsenheimer A, Kieckhaefer C, Rapp AM. Familiarity, empathy and comprehension of metaphors in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2020;291:113152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Rapp A, Felsenheimer AK, Langohr K, Klupp M. The comprehension of familiar and novel metaphoric meanings in schizophrenia: A pilot study. Front Psychol. 2018;8:2251. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02251.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Rapp AM, Langohr K, Mutschler DE, Wild B. Irony and Proverb Comprehension in Schizophrenia: Do Female Patients “Dislike” Ironic Remarks? Schizophr Res Treat. 2014;2014:841086. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/841086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Happé FGE. Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition. 1993;48(2):101–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90026-r.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Bambini V, Arcara G, Bosinelli F, et al. A leopard cannot change its spots: A novel pragmatic account of concretism in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia. 2020;139:107332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107332.

  83. Bleuler E. Dementia praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien. In: Deuticke F, editor. Handbuch der Psychiatrie. Leipzig: Wien; 1911. p. 412–3.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Harrow M, Adler D, Hanf E. Abstract and Concrete Thinking in Schizophrenia During the Prechronic Phases. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974;31(1):27–33. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1974.01760130013002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Searle JR. Indirect Speech Acts. In: Cole P, Morgan JL, editors. Speech Acts. Leiden: Brill; 1975. p. 59–82.

    Google Scholar 

  86. Tobin V, Israel M. Irony as a viewpoint phenomenon. In: Dancygier B, Sweetser E, editors. Viewpoint in Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2012. p. 25–46.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  87. Goldstein G. Neuropsychological heterogeneity in schizophrenia: a consideration of abstraction and problem-solving abilities. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1990;5(3):251–64.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  88. McDonald S, Flanagan S, Rollins J, Kinch J. TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2003;18(3):219–38. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001199-200305000-00001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Orme W, Bowersox L, Vanwoerden S, Fonagy P, Sharp C. The relation between epistemic trust and borderline pathology in an adolescent inpatient sample. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2019;6(1):13. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-019-0110-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Bender DS, Morey LC, Skodol AE. Toward a model for assessing level of personality functioning in DSM-5, part I: a review of theory and methods. J Pers Assess. 2011;93(4):332–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011.583808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Sharp C, Wright AGC, Christopher Fowler J, Christopher Frueh B, Allen JG, Oldham J, et al. The structure of personality pathology: Both general ('g’) and specific ('s’) factors? J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124(2):387–98. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Harari H, Shamay-Tsoory SG, Ravid M, Levkovitz Y. Double dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2010;175(3):277–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. New AS, Rot MA, Ripoll LH, Mercedes Perez-Rodriguez M, Lazarus S, Zipursky E, et al. Empathy and alexithymia in borderline personality disorder: Clinical and laboratory measures. J Personal Disord. 2012;26(5):660–75. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2012.26.5.660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Petersen R, Brakoulias V, Langdon R. An experimental investigation of mentalization ability in borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;64:12–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.10.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Morey LC. Development and initial evaluation of a self-report form of the DSM-5 level of personality functioning scale. Psychol Assess. 2017;29(10):1302. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000450.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Krueger RF, Hobbs KA. An Overview of the DSM-5 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders. Psychopathology. 2020;53:126–32. https://doi.org/10.1159/000508538.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Krueger RF, Derringer J, Markon KE, Watson D, Skodol AE. Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM-5. Psychol Med. 2012;42(9):1879–90. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002674.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Sperber D, Wilson D. Irony and the use-mention distinction. In: Cole P, editor. Radical pragmatics. New York: Academic Press; 1981. p. 296–318.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Caillies S, Gobin P, Obert A, Terrien S, Coutté A, Iakimova G, et al. Asymmetry of affect in verbal irony understanding: What about the N400 and P600 components? J Neurolinguistics. 2019;51:268–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2019.04.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Dews S, Winner E. Muting the meaning a social function of irony. Metapho Symb Act. 1995;10(1):3–19. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327868ms1001_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Pexman PM, Olineck KM. Does Sarcasm Always Sting? Investigating the Impact of Ironic Insults and Ironic Compliments. Discourse Process. 2002;33(3):199–217. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326950DP3303_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Weinbrecht A, Roepke S, Renneberg B. Fear of positive evaluation in borderline personality disorder. PLoS One. 2020;15(8):e0237944. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237944.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Reichenberger J, Eibl JJ, Pfaltz M, Wilhelm FH, Voderholzer U, Hillert A, et al. Don’t praise me, don’t chase me: emotional reactivity to positive and negative social-evaluative videos in patients with borderline personality disorder. J Personal Disord. 2016;31(1):75–89. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2016_30_238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Winter D, Herbert C, Koplin K, Schmahl C, Bohus M, Lis S. Negative Evaluation Bias for Positive Self-Referential Information in Borderline Personality Disorder. PLoS One. 2015;10(1):e0117083. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117083.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Wiesenfeller J, Flasbeck V, Brown EC, Brüne M. Approach and Avoidance Behavior in Female Patients With Borderline Personality Disorder. Front Behav Neurosci. 2020;14:588874. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.588874.

  106. Schneider I, Boll S, Volman I, et al. Oxytocin Normalizes Approach-Avoidance Behavior in Women With Borderline Personality Disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:120. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00120.

  107. Bateman A, Fonagy P, editors. Mentalization-Based Treatment for Personality Disorders. 1st ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2016.

  108. Foxhall M, Hamilton-Giachritsis C, Button K. The link between rejection sensitivity and borderline personality disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Clin Psychol. 2019;58(3):289–326. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Kleindienst N, Hauschild S, Liebke L, Thome J, Bertsch K, Hensel S, et al. A negative bias in decoding positive social cues characterizes emotion processing in patients with symptom-remitted Borderline Personality Disorder. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2019;6:17. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-019-0114-3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  110. Niedtfeld I, Renkewitz F, Mädebach A, Hillmann K, Kleindienst N, Schmahl C, et al. Enhanced memory for negative social information in borderline personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2020;129(5):480–91. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000540.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Vestergaard M, Kongerslev MT, Thomsen MS, Mathiesen BB, Harmer CJ, Simonsen E, et al. Women with borderline personality disorder show reduced identification of emotional facial expressions and a heightened negativity bias. J Personal Disord. 2020;34(5):677–98. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2019_33_409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  112. Veague HB, Hooley JM. Enhanced sensitivity and response bias for male anger in women with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(3):687–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.045.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Minzenberg MJ, Poole JH, Vinogradov S. Social-emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):468–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.03.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Niedtfeld I, Defiebre N, Regenbogen C, Mier D, Fenske S, Kirsch P, et al. facing the problem: impaired emotion recognition during multimodal social information processing in borderline personality disorder. J Personal Disord. 2017;31(2):273–88. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2016_30_248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Fenske S, Lis S, Liebke L, Niedtfeld I, Kirsch P, Mier D. Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: effects of emotional information on negative bias. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2015;2:10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-015-0031-z.

  116. Torgersen S, Kringlen E, Cramer V. The prevalence of personality disorders in a community sample. The Science of Mental Health: Volume 7. Personality Personality Disord. 2013;58(6):132–8. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.6.590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Ullrich S, Coid J. The age distribution of self-reported personality disorder traits in a household population. J Personal Disord. 2009;23(2):187–200. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.2.187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Linehan M. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. In: Diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders. New York: Guilford Press; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  119. Colston HL. Irony and Sarcasm. In: Attardo S, editor. The Routledge Handbook of Language and Humor. New York: Taylor and Francis; 2017. p. 234–49.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  120. Booth WC. A rhetoric of irony. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  121. Fuchs T. The phenomenology and development of social perspectives. Phenomenol Cogn Sci. 2013;12(4):655–83. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-012-9267-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  122. Gallese V. Bodily selves in relation: embodied simulation as second-person perspective on intersubjectivity. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2014;369(1644):20130177. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the participants for their involvement in the study and both actors who participated in the production of the stimuli. We acknowledge support by Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Tübingen.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. No funding was provided for the collection of this data or writing of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AF, CK and AR designed the study. AF and CF acquired the data. CF and AF prepared the data for analyzing. AF analyzed the data. AF wrote the article, which all authors reviewed and approved for publication. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne Katrin Felsenheimer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Ethics Commission of the Medical Faculty of the University Clinic of Tuebingen and complied with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the study.

Consent for publication

Both actors provided written informed consent for publication of video and picture material of the irony paradigm.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Example stimulus presentation of ironic criticism.

Additional file 2.

Dichotomous (literality) and rating scale (perceived intention) for each stimulus.

Additional file 3: Supplementary Table 1.

Results of the rmANOVA of the dependent variables irony detection accuracy and perceived intention based on literality, intention, and perspective of presented stimuli. There were no group differences and interactions regarding the perspective the participant adopted with regard to the written dialogues.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Felsenheimer, A.K., Kieckhäfer, C. & Rapp, A.M. Irony detection in patients with borderline personality disorder: an experimental study examining schizotypal traits, response biases and empathy. bord personal disord emot dysregul 9, 24 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-022-00194-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-022-00194-w

Keywords