Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Examining emotion regulation in binge-eating disorder

Abstract

Background

Inefficient mechanisms of emotional regulation appear essential in understanding the development and maintenance of binge-eating disorder (BED). Previous research focused mainly on a very limited emotion regulation strategies in BED, such as rumination, suppression, and positive reappraisal. Therefore, the aim of the study was to assess a wider range of emotional regulation strategies (i.e. acceptance, refocusing on planning, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, putting into perspective, self-blame, other-blame, rumination, and catastrophizing), as well as associations between those strategies and binge-eating-related beliefs (negative, positive, and permissive), and clinical variables (eating disorders symptoms, both anxiety, depressive symptoms, and alexithymia).

Methods

Women diagnosed with BED (n = 35) according to the DSM-5 criteria and healthy women (n = 41) aged 22–60 years were assessed using: the Eating Attitudes Test-26, the Eating Beliefs Questionnaire-18, the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20, the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Statistical analyses included: Student t - tests or Mann–Whitney U tests for testing group differences between BED and HC group, and Pearson’s r coefficient or Spearman’s rho for exploring associations between the emotion regulation difficulties and strategies, and clinical variables and binge-eating-related beliefs in both groups.

Results

The BED group presented with a significantly higher level of emotion regulation difficulties such as: nonacceptance of emotional responses, lack of emotional clarity, difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior, impulse control difficulties, and limited access to emotion regulation strategies compared to the healthy controls. Moreover, patients with BED were significantly more likely to use maladaptive strategies (rumination and self-blame) and less likely to use adaptive strategies (positive refocusing and putting into perspective). In the clinical group, various difficulties in emotion regulation difficulties occurred to be positively correlated with the level of alexithymia, and anxiety and depressive symptoms. Regarding emotion regulation strategies, self-blame and catastrophizing were positively related to anxiety symptoms, but solely catastrophizing was related to the severity of eating disorder psychopathology.

Conclusions

Our results indicate an essential and still insufficiently understood role of emotional dysregulation in BED. An especially important construct in this context seems to be alexithymia, which was strongly related to the majority of emotion regulation difficulties. Therefore, it might be beneficial to pay special attention to this construct when planning therapeutic interventions, as well as to the maladaptive emotion regulation strategies self-blame and catastrophizing, which were significantly related to BED psychopathology.

Introduction

Nowadays, binge-eating disorder (BED) is one of the most common diseases among feeding and eating disorders in DSM-5 classification (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition) [1]. Its lifetime prevalence among the general population from United States is estimated to about 2,03% [2]. BED manifests by recurrent episodes of eating large amounts of food in a discrete period (e. g. within 2 h), while experiencing a sense of lack of control [1]. The binge-eating episodes are characterized by: (a) rapid pace of eating, (b) eating until feeling extremely full, (c) eating without a feeling of physical hunger, (d) feeling guilty and disgusted about oneself afterwards [1]. Unlike bulimia nervosa (BN), inappropriate compensatory behaviors, such as purging, do not occur in BED [1]. A recent study found that some specific metacognitive beliefs about food and eating may be a maintaining factor in binge-eating symptoms [3]. Namely, three types of beliefs could be distinguished: negative beliefs (thoughts of having no control over one’s eating), positive beliefs (beliefs that eating improves one’s mood), and permissive beliefs (beliefs that one should allow oneself to binge-eat) [3]. Furthermore, the dysfunctional mechanisms of emotion regulation seem to be one of the most problematic issues of the clinical picture of BED [4]. Emotional regulation is a multidimensional construct that comprises all processes with which a person monitors, assesses, and modifies his or her emotional states [5]. It can be regarded as the ability to inhibit impulsive, dysfunctional reactions to strong emotions regardless of their valence or refocusing attention while experiencing intense emotions or self-restraining physiological stimulation that accompanies them [6].

The inability to regulate emotions adaptively seems to be important not only for the development but also for sustaining binge-eating [7, 8]. There are several models in which researchers have tried to explain these associations. One of the oldest models, the escape from self-awareness model by Heatherton and Baumeister [9], depicted overeating as an attempt to escape the aversive perception of oneself by narrowing the field of attention, which relates to the use of evasive coping strategies [10]. Fairburn’s transdiagnostic theory of eating disorders [11] raised an issue that intolerance of both positive and negative mood presents in some patients who overeat significantly hinders the effects of standard treatment (requiring additional interventions). However, the association between overeating episodes and the presence of negative emotions is particularly highlighted in the growing body of literature. The affect regulation model described by Polivy and Herman [12] sees the function of binges in alleviating negative affect, which secondarily reinforces a maladaptive coping means that becomes increasingly learned and automated. This model stays in line with extensive scientific literature, as follows: the systematic review by Leehr and coworkers [13], the meta-analysis by Cardi et al. [14] in conjunction with a significant part of the (electronic) diary studies [15,16,17,18]. According to the above-mentioned diary studies [15,16,17,18], negative affect in people with BED often becomes a trigger for overeating, which is not the case in people without this diagnosis, even in the case of obesity. However, Munsch et al. [18] state that it is not so much the occurrence or accumulation of negative emotions that is important, but their rapid increase, causing considerable tension, and consequently the breakdown of insufficiently efficient processes of regulating emotions. Giel et al. [19], on the other hand, stress the role of positive emotions associated with food consumption and the pleasure it may provide. Thus, making it more difficult to face impulses, which is particularly difficult in stressful situations [20]. There is a lack of consistency among empirical results on the impact of overeating on emotions and mood. According to some results, the affective state improves [15, 18], while, according to others, it worsens [10, 16, 21] after engaging in binge-eating.

Researchers posit that especially in case of coping with aversive emotions, overeating appears a consequence of a lack of more adaptive strategies of emotion regulation [7]. A wide range of possible strategies can be divided into adaptive and maladaptive ones, whereas the latter are related to psychopathology [22]. Examples of adaptive emotional regulation strategies are: cognitive reappraisal (i.e. attempts to change emotions by changing the interpretation of a given situation) [23], acceptance of the situation, refocusing on planning actions to deal with it, positive refocusing (thinking of other, pleasant matters instead of the negative event), positive reappraisal (treating a difficult situation as an opportunity for development), or putting into perspective (associating the relativity of a difficult event with other events) [24]. Maladaptive strategies of coping with difficult emotions would be, for example, overeating, [25], blaming oneself for a given situation or blaming others, catastrophic thinking, ruminating (persistent remembrance of a negative event and its consequences), [26] or suppression [22]. Rumination is a characteristic strategy engaged in the psychopathology of eating disorders, [22, 26,27,28] including binge-eating [22, 27]. Moreover, some studies found that individuals with BED tend to reappraise their emotions less in comparison to healthy controls [29, 30]. However, there is a paucity of research on certain emotion regulation strategies applied by BED patients.

The aim of the present study was to identify various emotion regulation difficulties in individuals diagnosed with BED in comparison to a healthy control group. Previous research (e. g [31, 29].) focused mainly on a very few emotion regulation strategies in BED, such as rumination, suppression, and positive reappraisal. Therefore, little is known so far about the other strategies. To address this gap in the literature, this is the first study, to the authors’ best knowledge, which allowed to assess such a wide range of emotion regulation strategies (including self-blame, catastrophizing, others-blame, putting into perspective, acceptance, positive refocusing, refocus on planning, positive reappraisal), used by BED patients in comparison to healthy controls. Moreover, this study aimed to analyze a spectrum of potential correlative interactions between applied strategies and difficulties in emotion regulation, and clinical variables (severity of eating disorders, severity of anxiety and depression symptoms, and level of alexithymia) in both subject groups, as depression, anxiety, and alexithymia are often comorbid with BED and determine its course [32, 33]. To the best of authors’ knowledge, this is also the first study exploring associations between binge- eating-related beliefs (positive, negative, and permissive) and various emotional regulation strategies and difficulties in individuals diagnosed with BED. This is especially important as these beliefs seems to be maintaining factors in BED [3].

In line with existing literature, we hypothesized that: (1) patients with BED report greater difficulties in emotion regulation than healthy controls, (2) patients with BED use more maladaptive strategies and less adaptive strategies than healthy controls, (3) various difficulties in emotion regulation and using of maladaptive strategies in BED group are positively correlated with the severity of eating disorders symptoms, depression, anxiety, and alexithymia, whereas (4) the use of adaptive strategies is negatively correlated with the severity of eating disorders symptoms, depression, anxiety, and alexithymia (5) abnormal food-related beliefs are associated with emotion regulation strategies and difficulties.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study involved 76 women aged 22 to 60 years. Thirty-five women were outpatients diagnosed with binge-eating disorder, assessed from January 2019 to April 2020. To be enrolled in the clinical group, the diagnosis had to be verified by the consultant psychiatrist (KK) based on a clinical interview and fulfilling DSM-5 criteria [1] for binge-eating disorder. To ensure a homogeneous sample, comorbid psychiatric diagnoses of personality disorders, bipolar affective disorder, psychoses, as well as habitual substance abuse were an exclusion factor as well as neurological disorders and pregnancy. The healthy control subjects consisted of 41 women recruited from a community setting, including university students and staff, properly matched to the age of the clinical group. They were recruited based upon clinical measures as well as clinical interviews according to DSM-5 to exclude the presence of any eating or other psychiatric disorders. Inclusion criteria for the healthy sample were: (1) normal body mass index (BMI) in the range from 18.5 to 24.9 kg/m2 and (2) no current diagnosis of any mental disorder according to DSM-5.

Procedure

Recruitment took place in both outpatient obesity clinic and mental health outpatient settings in Warsaw, in local Anonymous Eaters Association, and via websites for people with binge-eating problems using a study project advert. Every participant signed the informed consent for participation in the study. There was no remuneration for participating in the study. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Institutional Review Board; number 05/2019).

Recruited participants were asked to complete two questionnaires to verify the presence and severity of the eating disorders psychopathology, i.e. the Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26 [34, 35];) and the Eating Beliefs Questionnaire (EBQ-18 [3];). The participants were then asked to provide their weight and height to calculate the BMI and to complete the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS [36, 37];), the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20 [38, 39];), the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ [24, 40];), and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS [41];). The control group underwent the same test procedure.

Questionnaire measures

Eating Attitude Test (EAT-26)

Nutrition problems were first evaluated using the Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26 [34]; Polish adaptation by Rogoza et al. [35]); scale reliability (α = 0.80). This is a widely used 26-item standardized self-report measurement, consisting of the following subscales: dieting, oral control, bulimia, and food preoccupation. Each item is rated on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 6 (always). A score of above 20 suggests abnormal eating behaviors or a high level of concerns about weight and dieting.

Eating Beliefs Questionnaire (EBQ-18)

The Eating Beliefs Questionnaire (EBQ-18 [3]) (internal consistency α = 0.94), was used to measure negative, positive, and permissive beliefs about food, which, according to the authors of the tool [3] are the underlying factor of binge-eating. Participants provide a rating on a 5-point scale of how strongly they agree with the statement from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicate increased severity in eating psychopathology. The tool in its original version has good test-retest reliability at an interval of 2 weeks (scale-dependent r ranging between 0.74 and 0.90).

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)

The level of anxiety and depression symptoms was measured with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS [36]; Polish validation by Nezlek [37]), which is a commonly used tool consisting of 14 items grouped into two subscales corresponding to the anxiety component (7 items) and the depression component (7 items). Each item is rated on a 4 point rating scale (0–3). The result can be calculated separately for each scale. The Polish version of the tool has very good internal consistency (α = 0.88) [42].

Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20)

The level of alexithymia was measured with the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20 [38];; Polish adaptation by Ścigała et al. [39]) consisting of 20 items grouped into three subscales: Difficulty Identifying Feeling (DIF), Difficulty Describing Feeling (DDF) and Externally Oriented Thinking (EOT). Participants rate each item on a rating scale from 1 to 5, with a higher score indicating higher degrees of alexithymia. The Polish version of the tool has good psychometric properties (α = 0.82–0.86) [39].

Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS)

The evaluation of the regulation of emotions was performed with the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS [41];; Polish translation by Czub [43]), which is a self-reporting tool consisting of 36 items that create the following dimensions: nonacceptance of emotional responses, difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior, impulse control difficulties, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, lack of emotional awareness, and lack of emotional clarity. Questions are answered by specifying the frequency of the experience on a 5-point rating scale (0 never to 5 all the time). The English version of the tool has high internal consistency (α = 0.93) and good test-retest reliability over a period ranging from 4 to 8 weeks (ρI = .88, p < 0.01) [40].

Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ)

The use of specific strategies, both adaptive and maladaptive, was studied using the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ [24];) in the Polish adaptation by Marszał-Wiśniewska and Fajkowska [40]. The adaptive strategies comprise acceptance, positive refocusing, refocusing on planning, positive reappraisal, and putting into perspective, while the maladaptive strategies include self-blame, other-blame, catastrophizing, and rumination. The complete tool consists of 36 items. Participants rate the frequency of the experience on 5-point rating scales (1 almost never to 5 almost always). The Polish version is characterized by good internal consistency (α for 8 out of 9 strategies is 0.70–0.87, and only for acceptance strategy it is 0.52) and satisfactory theoretical accuracy relating to the correlations between the strategy of emotional regulation and emotional, temperamental, and volitional properties [40].

Statistical analyses

To test the hypotheses, statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics software version 25 (IBM, Armonk, NY). It was used to calculate basic descriptive statistics, normal distribution tests, and, depending on their results, t-tests or Mann–Whitney U tests for testing group differences. Correlation analyses both in the BED group and the healthy controls were performed using Pearson’s r coefficient or its non-parametric equivalent of Spearman’s rho in case of non-fullfilment of the requirements. To account for multiple comparisons with the problem of alpha-error inflation, we applied the Bonferroni correction.

Results

Sample characteristics

Sociodemographic variables

No statistically significant age difference was observed between the groups. There was a significant difference in BMI between subject groups indicating overweight on the verge of obesity in BED group and normal body weight in healthy control (Table 1).

Table 1 Sociodemographic data in both subject groups

Clinical variables

The student t-test showed that women in the clinical group were characterized by significantly higher levels of eating disorders symptoms (overall EAT-26 result and its subscales dieting and bulimia and food preoccupation), alexithymia (mean global score, difficulty in identifying feeling, difficulty in describing feeling), anxiety, and depressive symptoms than control group. Additionally, in the group with BED, a higher number of positive, negative and permissive food-related beliefs were observed. These differences were highly significant (p < 0.001), also in the case of the global score. The observed effect sizes for all variables were large (except the difficulty in describing feelings to others, with a medium effect size). Detailed values of the discussed analyses are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 The differences between the BED group and healthy controls in the EAT-26, EBQ-18, TAS-20, and HADS tool dimension levels

Emotion regulation

In the DERS, women in the clinical group were characterized by significantly higher levels of difficulties in emotion regulation, nonacceptance of emotional responses, difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior, impulse control difficulties, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and lack of emotional clarity than in the control group. Solely the subscale of lack of emotional awareness revealed non-significant differences. The magnitude of effects for all statistically significant differences were substantial. The exact values of the discussed analyses are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 The differences between the BED group and healthy controls in the DERS tool dimension levels

In the CERQ, women in the healthy control group had significantly higher levels of positive refocusing and putting into perspective than in the clinical group. On the other hand, women in the BED group were characterized by significantly higher levels of rumination and self-blame than those in the control group. The observed effect sizes measured by Cohen’s d coefficients ranged from 0.66 to 1.34, indicating moderate to strong effects. Detailed values of the discussed tests are presented in Table 4.

Table 4 The differences between the BED group and healthy controls in the CERQ tool dimension levels

Correlation analyses

The analysis of the correlation in the clinical and control group between the DERS and CERQ dimensions and clinical variables using the Pearson’s r or non-parametric Spearman’s rho factor revealed the presence of moderately strong, strong, and very strong correlations. Due to the large size of the tables and the amount of statistically significant relations, all results can be found in the Appendix – Table A1, A2 (BED group), and Table A3, A4 (HC group).

Difficulties in emotion regulation scale

In the BED group, several statistically significant positive correlations between the subscales of DERS and the clinical variables were observed. Difficulties in emotion regulation were very strongly correlated with alexithymia (p < 0.001), and its subscale: difficulty in describing feeling (p < 0.001), and very strongly with difficulty in identifying feeling (p < 0.001). Also, strong relationships were detected between difficulties in emotion regulation and anxiety (p < 0.001), and depression (p < 0.001). Nonacceptance of emotional responses was found to be strongly correlated with alexithymia (p < 0.001) and its subscale: difficulty in describing feeling (p < 0.001), anxiety (p < 0.001), and depression (p < 0.001), as well as moderately with difficulty in identifying feeling (p = 0.004) (also an alexithymia subscale). There was a moderate correlation between difficulties engaging in goal directed behavior and intensity of depressive symptoms (p = 0.007). Impulse control difficulties were moderately correlated with alexithymia (p = 0.005) and its subscale: difficulty in identifying feeling (p = 0.007), and strongly with difficulty in describing feeling (p < 0.001). Lack of emotional awareness was found to be strongly correlated with alexithymia (p = 0.002). Strong relationships were also detected between limited access to emotion regulation strategies and alexithymia (p < 0.001) and its subscales: difficulty in identifying feeling (p = 0.001), and difficulty in describing feeling (p < 0.001), anxiety (p < 0.001), and depression (p < 0.001). Finally, lack of emotional clarity correlated very strongly with alexithymia (p < 0.001) and its subscales: difficulty in identifying feeling (p < 0.001) and difficulty in describing feeling (p < 0.001), and strongly with anxiety (p < 0.001).

In the healthy controls group, only three dimensions of the DERS were found to be significantly and positively correlated with clinical measures. Difficulties in emotion regulation were moderately correlated with difficulty in identifying feeling (p = 0.006), and moderately with intensity of depressive symptoms (p = 0.003). A moderate correlation was also found between nonacceptance of emotional responses and severity of depressive symptoms (p = 0.003). Finally, lack of emotional clarity was strongly correlated with alexithymia (p < 0.001) and its subscales: difficulty in identifying feeling (p < 0.001), and difficulty in describing feeling (p < 0.001), and with intensity of depressive symptoms (p < 0.001).

Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire

In the BED group, a strong positive relationship was revealed between self-blame (CERQ subscale) and anxiety (p = 0.002). A strong positive correlation was also observed also between the catastrophizing subscale and intensity of eating disorders (i.e. the global EAT-26 score; p = 0.002, and its subscale dieting; p < 0.001). The correlation between catastrophizing and anxiety was moderate (p = 0.004).

In the healthy controls group, the only statistically significant correlation between the CERQ and the clinical variables was found between self-blame and externally oriented thinking (a moderate negative correlation, p = 0.005).

Discussion

The main aim of this study was to evaluate strategies of emotional regulation in women struggling with binge-eating disorder. According to the main hypothesis raised, patients with BED were expected to experience more difficulties in this domain and to use more maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and fewer adaptive strategies than healthy women in the control group. Indeed, a higher nonacceptance of emotional responses, lack of emotional clarity, difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior, impulse control difficulties, and limited access to emotion regulation strategies were observed in the BED group compared to healthy women. It should be noted that the effect was strong in each case. This is consistent with the literature reports highlighting deficits in these areas in patients with BED [7, 8, 17, 20, 32]. Interestingly, however, a difference was observed between the groups assessed in this study regarding the nonacceptance of emotional responses (DERS) subscale, whereas it was not observed in the acceptance subscale understood as acceptance of negative events (CERQ). This indicates the multifaceted construct which acceptance is. The lack of difficulties in agreeing to negative events may result from the presence of variables that were not considered in this study, such as internal attribution style or low self-assessment. They may influence the perception of negative events in life as completely natural, consistent with the negative way of thinking about oneself [44]. However, this observation would need to be verified in subsequent studies.

Greater level of impulse control difficulties seems to follow other studies [45, 46] reporting high negative urgency (the tendency to act rashly when distressed) in individuals with BED. These findings highlight the potential value of focusing on this facet of impulsivity, i.e. negative urgency in treatments for BED, as it may play a role between negative affect and binge-eating [32]. By assessing a wide range of emotion regulation strategies, this study showed that patients with BED use more maladaptive strategies to regulate emotions than healthy women, which is consistent with the current research (i.e. [29, 30]). Healthy controls applied positive refocusing and putting into perspective more often than patients with BED did. This is consistent with Kelly, Lydecker and Mazzeo’s [47] study results performed in a large group of individuals (N = 419). Above mentioned authors [47] demonstrated that the use of more positive strategies is associated with a lower frequency of binge-eating. When it comes to negative strategies, in the present study women from clinical group were more inclinated to use rumination and self-blame (strong effects) than healthy women. These two maladaptive strategies have been raised by other authors. For example, Svaldi et al. [8] noticed that in comparison to healthy women, BED patients’ self-blame more often. It might be related to the feeling of shame, which is characteristic for individuals with BED [1]. Patients with this disorder may be ashamed of e.g. their body weight or having no control over food intake, and perceive its as one’s own fault [1]. However, by far the strongest evidence in the research literature is provided for using rumination as a strategy by BED patients (i.e. [43, 22, 27, 28, 48]). In one longitudinal study conducted by Nolen-Hoeksem et al. [25], the use of this strategy by teenage girls even predicted the development of binge-eating in the future. Therefore, the search for further relationships between the applied strategies and clinical symptoms accompanying BED (i.e. severity of eating disorder, and symptoms of anxiety, depression, and alexithymia) seems justified.

In present study, many strong, statistically significant relationships were observed in the BED group between higher difficulties in emotion regulation (nonacceptance of emotional responses, impulse control difficulties, lack of emotional awareness, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, lack of emotional clarity), and higher level of alexithymia (and its subscales: difficulty in identifying and describing feelings). What is important, in the healthy controls, only one aspect (lack of emotional clarity) was significantly related to alexithymia. Therefore, one might hypothesize that problems in identifying and describing feelings may play an essential role in difficulties in emotion regulation observed in people with BED. Although this hypothesis needs to be verified in further research, some other studies appear supportive of it. For example, in a study of Brown et al. [49] conducted with patients with eating disorders (anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa), admission alexithymia was related to levels and change in global emotion dysregulation, access to emotion regulation strategies, and impulse control difficulties. Moreover, in the anorexia nervosa group, lower levels of alexithymia coexisted with lower levels of emotion dysregulation [49]. Also, the findings of Pandey et al. [50] showed that alexithymia was related to greater emotion regulation difficulties, which were additionally associated with severe health-related problems. There are no studies examining relationships between alexithymia and emotion regulation difficulties in BED individuals to the authors’ best knowledge.

The present study also revealed the associations between greater difficulties in emotion regulation in the BED group (nonacceptance of emotional responses, lack of emotional clarity, difficulties engaging in goal- directed behavior, limited access to emotion regulation strategies) and higher intensity of anxiety and depressive symptoms. Interestingly, in the healthy control group, this relationship was weaker and occurred only concerning the first two of the mentioned subscales. It might be hypothesized that anxiety and depression could be, at least, partially the consequence of difficulties in emotion regulation. This thesis could be supported by the results obtained by Gonçalves and colleagues [51]. In this cross-sectional and longitudinal study, the authors showed that overall difficulties in emotion regulation and limited access to emotion regulation strategies predict the intensity of depressive symptoms. Therefore, they recommend the inclusion of interventions aimed at improving emotional regulation in the prevention of depression. As far as anxiety is concerned, Menezes and colleagues [52] showed that improvement in emotion regulation processes in general emotion dysregulation and difficulties in regulatory strategies after 6 weeks of training was related to reduction of trait anxiety. In other studies, associations between lower difficulties in emotion regulation and lower anxiety in patients with BED were also confirmed [53, 54]. However, after Bonferroni correction, no statistically significant correlations remained in the clinical group between eating disorders psychopathology and difficulties in emotion regulation. Considering that before the application of the Bonferroni correction, the vast majority of the mentioned correlations exceed the threshold of statistical significance (p < 0.05), one might hypothesize that in the case of a larger number of participants, these relationships would remain significant also after Bonferroni correction. Additionally, both depressive and anxiety symptoms are constructs that focus to a greater extent on emotional aspects of functioning than in the case of eating disorders psychopathology which is more multidimensional (very important role of behavioral components). That could make it understandable that the correlations between anxiety and depressive symptoms and emotion regulation become more pronounced. It also seems possible that anxiety and/or depressive symptoms could potentially be mediators / moderators of the relationship between emotion regulation and eating disorders psychopathology. However, further research with a greater number of participants is necessary to verify those hypotheses and deepen the knowledge about above mentioned relationships.

When it comes to using strategies of emotion regulation by individuals with BED, the present study revealed that the higher level of anxiety was related to higher self-blame and catastrophizing. The latter was also associated with a higher level of eating disorders pathology. Interestingly, those dependencies were not observed in healthy controls, suggesting that they are specific for psychopathology accompanying BED. It is possible that - as it was obtained in a study by Martin and Dahlen [55] - catastrophizing and self-blame would be factors that enable the prediction of anxiety symptoms. If so, decreasing maladaptive and developing new, adaptive strategies of emotion regulation would be an important factor in dealing with extended anxiety in people with BED. However, further research is necessary to explore the direction of these relationships. It is especially important in relation to catastrophizing as it was the only emotion regulation strategy strongly related to the severity of eating disorder symptoms.

Limitations and future directions

When considering the study results, some caution should be taken about the limitations which, despite efforts, could not be avoided. The final size of the clinical group due to the desire to gather a homogeneous group of patients with BED is relatively small. However, the effect sizes were mostly substantial; thus, our study seems sufficiently powered. Additionally, the correlational nature of the study does not allow for drawing conclusions about causality, but only for creating hypotheses. Further research using a larger sample is necessary to enable more advanced statistical analyses, which could be very interesting especially in reference to the alexithymia construct. Inclusion of men in the study would also allow conclusions to be drawn about the differences between the sexes in the clinical picture of BED. As this study is mainly based on retrospective self-report measures with the possibility of recall biases, it also seems appropriate to design studies using methods that eliminate those, e.g. e-diary studies, and studies conducted in the natural environment of BED individuals (i.e. by Ecological Sampling Method). Finally, since the changes in emotions are of a dynamic nature, it would be important, apart from cross-sectional studies, to conduct longitudinal and prospective studies that would enable the observation of this phenomenon.

Conclusions

Overall, the results obtained in this study are consistent with previous findings on the complex BED psychopathology, indicating an important and still insufficiently understood role of emotional dysregulation in the clinical picture of this disorder. Several significant relationships between various difficulties in emotion regulation and alexithymia suggests the importance of this construct and support the increasingly popular position that therapeutic work with people struggling with BED should be aimed not only at difficulties in emotion regulation themselves, but first of all at more basic abilities – i.e. recognizing and naming emotions, increasing their clarity, and learning to accept them [56]. Among the analyzed strategies, only catastrophizing and self-blame were related to the psychopathology observed in the BED group while solely catastrophizing turned out to be related to symptoms of eating disorders. Therefore, it might be beneficial to pay special attention to them while planning therapeutic interventions in BED.

Availability of data and materials

Data access can be requested from the corresponding author on reasonable demand.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association Press; 2013. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Cossrow N, Pawaskar M, Witt EA, Ming EE, Victor TW, Herman BK, et al. Estimating the prevalence of binge eating disorder in a community sample from the United States: comparing DSM-IV-TR and DSM-5 criteria. J Clin Psychiatry. 2016;77(8):968–74. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.15m10059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Burton AL, Mitchison D, Hay P, Donnelly B, Thornton C, Russell J, et al. Beliefs about binge eating: psychometric properties of the eating beliefs questionnaire (EBQ-18) in eating disorder, obese, and community samples. Nutrients. 2018;10(9):1306. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10091306.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Fairburn CG. Overcoming binge eating. 2nd ed. the proven program to learn why you binge and how you can stop. New York: The Guilford Press; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Garnefski N, Kraaij V. The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2007;23(3):141–9. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Linehan M. Terapia dialektyczno-behawioralna (DBT). Trening umiejętności. Kraków: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego; 2016. Polish

    Google Scholar 

  7. Brockmeyer T, Skunde M, Wu M, Bresslein E, Rudofsky G, Herzog W, et al. Difficulties in emotion regulation across the spectrum of eating disorders. Compr Psychiatry. 2014;55(3):565–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.12.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Svaldi J, Griepenstroh J, Tuschen-Caffier B, Ehring T. Emotion regulation deficits in eating disorders: a marker of eating pathology or general psychopathology? Psychiatry Res. 2012;197(1–2):103–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Heatherton TF, Baumeister RF. Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychol Bull. 1991;110(1):86–108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Haedt-Matt A, Keel P. Revisiting the affect regulation model of binge eating: a meta analysis of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(4):660–81. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023660.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Fairburn CG. Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York: Guilford Press; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Polivy J, Herman CP. Etiology of binge eating: psychological mechanisms. In: Fairburn CG, Wilson GT, editors. Binge eating: nature, assessment, and treatment. New York: The Guilford Press; 1993. p. 173–205.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Leehr EJ, Krohmer K, Schag K, Dresle T, Zipfel S, Giel KE. Emotion regulation model in binge eating disorder and obesity-a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;49:125–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Cardi V, Leppanen J, Treasure J. The effects of negative and positive mood induction on eating behaviour: a meta-analysis of laboratory studies in the healthy population and eating and weight disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;57:299–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Berg KC, Crosby RD, Cao L, Crow SJ, Engel SG, Wonderlich SA, et al. Negative affect prior to and following overeating-only, loss of control eating-only, and binge eating episodes in obese adults. Int J Eat Disord. 2015;48(6):641–53. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22401.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Hilbert A, Tuschen-Caffier B. Maintenance of binge eating through negative mood: a naturalistic comparison of binge eating disorder and bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2007;40(6):521–30. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Keating L, Mills JS, Rawana JS. Momentary predictors of binge eating: an attachment perspective. Eat Behav. 2019;32:44–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2018.12.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Munsch S, Meyer AH, Quartier V, Wilhelm FH. Binge eating in binge eating disorder: a break-down of emotion regulatory process? Psychiatry Res. 2011;195(3):118–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Giel KE, Teufel M, Junne F, Zipfel S, Schag K. Food-related impulsivity in obese and binge eating disorder – a systematic update of the evidence. Nutrients. 2017;9(11):e1170. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9111170.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Monell E, Clinton D, Birgegard A. Emotion dysregulation and eating disorders – associations with diagnostic presentation and key symptoms. Int J Eat Disord. 2018;51(8):921–30. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Stein R, Kenardy J, Wiseman CV, Dounchis JZ, Arnow BA, Wilfley DE. What’s driving the binge in binge eating disorder? A prospective examination of precursors and consequences. Int J Eat Disord. 2007;40(3):195–203. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: a transdiagnostic examination. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(10):974–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Giuliani N, Gross JJ. Reappraisal. In: Sander D, Scherer K, editors. Oxford companion to the affective sciences. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P. CERQ. Manual for the use of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Leiderdrop: DATEC; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Schaefer LM, Smith KE, Anderson LM, Cao L, Crosby RD, Engel SG, et al. The role of affect in the maintenance of binge-eating disorder: evidence from an ecological momentary assessment study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2020;129(4):387–96. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000517.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Stice E, Wade E, Bohon C. Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116(1):198–207. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Wang SB, Lydecker JA, Grilo CM. Rumination in patients with binge-eating disorder and obesity: associations with eating-disorder psychopathology and weight-bias internalization. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2017;25(2):98–103. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2499.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Dondzilo L, Rieger E, Palermo R, Byrne S, Bell J. Association between rumination factors and eating disorder behaviours in young women. Adv Eat Disord. 2016;4(1):84–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/21662630.2015.1118642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Svaldi J, Caffier D, Tuschen-Caffier B. Emotion suppression but not reappraisal increases desire to binge in women with binge eating disorder. Psychother Psychosom. 2010;79(3):188–90. https://doi.org/10.1159/000296138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Danner UN, Sternheim L, Evers C. The importance of distinguishing between the different eating disorder (sub) types when assessing emotion regulation strategies. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(3):727–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Smith KE, Mason TB, Anderson NL, Lavender JM. Unpacking cognitive emotion regulation in eating disorder psychopathology: the differential relationships between rumination, thought suppression, and eating disorder symptoms among men and women. Eat Behav. 2019;32:95–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2019.01.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Dingemans A, Danner U, Parks M. Emotion regulation in binge eating disorder: a review. Nutrients. 2017;9(11):1274. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9111274.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Westwood H, Kerr-Gaffney J, Stahl D, Tchanturia K. Alexithymia in eating disorders: systematic review and meta-analyses of studies using Toronto alexithymia scale. J Psychosom Res. 2017;99:66–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Garner DM, Olmsted MP, Bohr Y, Garfinkel PE. The eating attitudes test: psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychol Med. 1982;12(4):871–8. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291700049163.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Rogoza R, Brytek-Matera A, Garner D. Analysis of the EAT-26 in a non-clinical sample. Arch Psychiatry Psychother. 2016;18(2):54–8. https://doi.org/10.12740/APP/63647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67(6):361–70. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Nezlek JB, Rusanowska M, Holas P, Krejtz I. The factor structure of a polish language version of the hospital anxiety depression scale (HADS). Curr Psychol. 2019;40(5):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12144-019-0164-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Taylor GJ, Bagby M, Parker JD. The revised Toronto alexithymia scale: some reliability, validity, and normative data. Psychother Psychosom. 1992;57(1–2):34–41. https://doi.org/10.1159/000288571.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Ścigała DK, Zdankiewicz-Ścigała E, Bedyńska S, Kokoszka A. Psychometric properties and Configural invariance of the polish – language version of the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale in non-clinical and alcohol addict persons. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1241. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Marszał-Wiśniewska M, Fajkowska M. Właściwości psychometryczne Kwestionariusza Poznawczej Regulacji Emocji (Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; CERQ)-wyniki badań na polskiej próbie. Stud Psychol. 2010;49(1):19–39.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26(1):41–54. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:joba.0000007455.08539.94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Watrowski R, Rohde A. Validation of the polish version of the hospital anxiety and depression scale in three populations of gynecologic patients. Arch Med Sci. 2014;10(3):517–24. https://doi.org/10.5114/aoms.2013.36520.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Czub T. Opis Kwestionariusza trudności w regulacji emocji – DERS/PL. Poznań: Instytut Psychologii Uniwersytetu im. Adama Mickiewicza (maszynopis niepublikowany); 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Baumeister RF, editor. Self-esteem: the puzzle of low self-regard. New York: Plenum Press; 1993. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-8956-9.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  45. Kelly NR, Cotter EW, Mazzeo SE. Examining the role of distress tolerance and negative urgency in binge eating behavior among women. Eat Behav. 2014;15(3):483–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.06.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Fischer S, Smith GT, Cyders MA. Another look at impulsivity: a meta-analytic review comparing specific dispositions to rash action in their relationship to bulimic symptoms. Clin Psychol Rev. 2008;28(8):1413–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Kelly NR, Lydecker JA, Mazzeo SE. Positive cognitive coping strategies and binge eating in college women. Eat Behav. 2012;13(3):289–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.03.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Smith KE, Mason TB, Lavender JM. Rumination and eating disorder psychopathology: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2018;61:9–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.03.004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Brown TA, Avery JC, Jones MD, Anderson LK, Wierenga CE, Kaye WH. The impact of alexithymia on emotion dysregulation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa over time. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2018;26(2):150–5. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Pandey R, Saxena P, Dubey A. Emotion regulation difficulties in alexithymia and mental health. Eur J Psychol Educ. 2011;7(4):604–23. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v7i4.155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Gonçalves SF, Chaplin TM, Turpyn CC, Niehaus CE, Curby TW, Sinha R, et al. Difficulties in emotion regulation predict depressive symptom trajectory from early to middle adolescence. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2019;50(4):618–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-019-00867-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Menezes CB, Bizarro L. Effects of focused meditation on difficulties in emotion regulation and trait anxiety. Psychol Neurosci. 2015;8(3):350–65. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Salters-Pedneault K, Roemer L, Tull MT, Rucker L, Mennin DS. Evidence of broad deficits in emotion regulation associated with chronic worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Cognit Ther Res. 2006;30(4):469–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9055-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Tull MT, Barrett HM, McMillan ES, Roemer L. A preliminary investigation of the relationship between emotion regulation difficulties and posttraumatic stress symptoms. Behav Ther. 2007;38(3):303–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.10.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Martin RC, Dahlen ER. Cognitive emotion regulation in the prediction of depression, anxiety, stress, and anger. Personal Individ Differ. 2005;39(7):1249–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Aloi M, Rania M, Caroleo M, De Fazio P, Segura-García C. Social cognition and emotional functioning in patients with binge eating disorder. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2017;25(3):172–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, A.W., K.K..; methodology, K.K.; data collection, A.W., B.K; analyses, A.W.; interpretation of data, A.W., B.K., K.K; writing—original draft preparation, A.W., B.K., writing—review and editing, A.W., B.K., P.S., K.K.; supervision, K.K. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Barbara Kostecka or Philip S. Santangelo.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics and Bioethics Committee of the Cardinal Stefan Wyszynski University in Warsaw (05/2019; date of approval: 12.06.2019). Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table A1.

Correlation between the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) dimensions and clinical variables in the BED group. Table A2. Correlation between the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) dimensions and clinical variables in the BED group. Table A3. Correlation between the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) dimensions and clinical variables in the control group. Table A4. Correlation between the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) dimensions and clinical variables in the control group.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Walenda, A., Kostecka, B., Santangelo, P.S. et al. Examining emotion regulation in binge-eating disorder. bord personal disord emot dysregul 8, 25 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-021-00166-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-021-00166-6

Keywords