Skip to main content

Impaired mentalizing in depression and the effects of borderline personality disorder on this relationship

Abstract

Background

Mentalizing, the ability to understand the self and others as well as behaviour in terms of intentional mental states, is impaired in Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD). Evidence for mentalizing deficits in other mental disorders, such as depression, is less robust and these links have never been explored while accounting for the effects of BPD on mentalizing. Additionally, it is unknown whether BPD symptoms might moderate any relationship between depressive symptoms and mentalizing.

Methods

Using multivariate regression modelling on cross-sectional data obtained from a sample of 274 participants recruited from clinical settings, we investigated the association between mentalizing impairment and depression and examined whether this was moderated by the presence and number of concurrent BPD symptoms, while adjusting for socio-demographic confounders.

Results

Impaired mentalizing was associated with depressive symptoms, after adjustment for socio-demographic confounders and BPD symptoms (p = 0.002, β = − 0.18). BPD symptoms significantly moderated the association between impaired mentalizing and depressive symptoms (p = 0.003), with more severe borderline symptoms associated with a stronger effect of poor mentalization on increased depressive symptoms.

Conclusion

Mentalizing impairments occur in depression even after adjusting for the effect of BPD symptoms. Our findings help further characterise mentalizing impairments in depression, as well as the moderating effect of BPD symptoms on this association.. Further longitudinal work is required to investigate the direction of association.

Introduction

Mentalizing refers to the ability to understand behaviours and actions in oneself and others in terms of internal mental states. Skilful mentalization is accurate, coherent, rich in depth and flexible [1] and is thought to develop best within stable and secure relationships within childhood [2]. Therefore, we might expect mentalization to be compromised across a broad range of psychiatric disorders reflecting impaired development in the face of stress [3]. Indeed, Mentalization has been found to be impaired in individuals with borderline personality disorder (BPD) [4, 5]. Indeed, it has been proposed that mentalizing deficits may underlie key features of BPD [6]. For example, elevated sensitivity to interpersonal rejection, a core feature of BPD, could be considered a failure of adequately mentalizing the intentions of others [7]. Inadequate mentalization may therefore underlie excessive responses such as hostile behaviours in response to rejection, due to a misreading of both intent and threat [8]. Individuals with BPD are particularly prone to pre-mentalistic states, such as psychic equivalence (regarding negative thoughts as indicating or being equivalent to a negative reality) when they experience strong negative affect [9, 10]. These deficits in mentalizing are thought to arise from disordered attachment and negative experiences of social communication [11].

The occurrence of mentalizing deficits in major depressive disorder (MDD) has been hypothesized [12] and previously reported, although the evidence is less clear cut than in the case of BPD [9]. While two studies reported mentalizing deficits in psychiatric inpatients with depression compared to healthy controls [13, 14], another found no difference in reflective functioning, an operatilization of mentalizing, between chronically depressed patients receiving psychotherapy and healthy controls [15]. Poor mentalizing could precipitate low mood by increasing sensitivity to interpersonal rejection and failure to calibrate negative self-representation through the view others may hold about oneself [11].

Major Depressive disorder (MDD) and BPD often occur concurrently [16] and it is estimated that over 80% of individuals with BPD experience an episode of MDD over the course of their life [17]. From a clinical perspective, the occurrence of this co-morbidity is important because BPD co-morbidity has been shown to reduce the chance of MDD remitting [16] and is more common in chronic depression.

Impairments in mentalizing therefore appear to be a transdiagnostic construct that may contribute to deficits across both MDD and BPD. Poor mentalisation may be particularly harmful in BPD where mentalizing deficits interface with pre-existing negative self-representation. This increases vulnerability to pre-mentalizing modes, such as psychic equivalence that may uniquely contribute to the onset of depressive symptoms [10].

In this study, we therefore set out to investigate whether there is an association between impairment in mentalizing and depressive symptoms and furthermore, to test whether borderline symptoms moderated this relationship, hypothesizing that the links between poor mentalisation and depression should be stronger in those with higher levels of BPD symptoms.

Methods

Recruitment

Ethics approval was gained for the collection and use of all participants’ data (Research Ethics Committee Wales, 12/WA/0283).

Potential participants were aged 18–60 and consisted of three samples all recruited from the London area: i) a sample of 83 patients with BPD, recruited from specialist clinical services for people with BPD and confirmed via structured clinical interview (SCID II); ii) a sample of 119 patients with affective disorder, recruited from local NHS psychological treatment services; iii) a sample of 72 healthy adult control participants recruited from a range of community settings (online, notice boards, universities, and job centres). In terms of eligibility criteria, all participants were required to be over the age of 18 years and fluent in English. Individuals were excluded if they were psychotic, had a learning disability, or if they had a neurological disorder.

All participants were assessed at the offices of University College London. Participants were remunerated for their time and expenses incurred.

Table 1 presents the sample’s demographics.

Table 1 Baseline Characteristics of Sample

Questionnaires

Mentalizing was assessed with the Mentalisation Questionnaire (MZQ) a 15-item, previously validated self-report questionnaire [5]. Scores on each item range from 0 to 4 on each item with lower scores indicating worse mentalizing, with a possible range of scores from 0 to 60.

Depression was assessed with the Becks Depression Inventory -II (BDI-II) [18], a well validated and widely used measure of depression. The BDI-II consists of 21 questions on which participants are scored from 0 to 3 for a maximum of 63. Standard clinical groupings used from this are; minimal depression (BDI-II < 14), mild depression (BDI-II 14–19), moderate depression (BDI-II 20–28) and severe depression (BDI-II > 29).

Borderline personality traits were assessed with the Personality Assessment Inventory – Borderline sub-section (PAI-BOR) [19]. This has been validated across a wide range of ages and genders [20]. A total PAI-BOR raw score of greater than 38 indicates significant BPD features, while a score of 60 or more indicates levels of functioning typically associated with a diagnosis of BPD.

Statistical analysis

Software package STATA v15 was used for statistical analysis [21].

Simple descriptive statistics were used to characterise the sample. Visual examination of scatter plots and residual plots were undertaken to check linear regression assumptions. Linear regression models were used to model the association between mentalizing impairment and depressive symptoms. Simple linear regression, without adjustment, was used to obtain univariate associations between mentalizing, depressive symptoms and BPD symptoms. The association between depressive symptoms and mentalizing was then assessed in a fully adjusted model accounting for age, gender, household income, ethnicity, years in education, current employment, and BPD symptoms. To test whether BPD symptoms moderated the association between mentalizing and depression, we fitted an interaction term between BPD and MZQ score and used a likelihood ratio test to assess the statistical significance of the interaction term.

Results

Baseline characteristics

Two hundred and 74 participants had complete data for age, gender, BDI-II, MZQ and PAIS scoring, 245 of whom also had complete data for socio-demographic characteristics. The total pooled sample was predominantly female (73%), poor (42% - household income <£20,000) and white (64%) with a mean age of 30.3 (SD = 10.8) years. The mean PAI-BOR score in the pooled sample was 38.8 (SD = 16.5) and mean BDI-II score was 25.9 (SD = 12.1) (Table 1).

Univariate associations

Worsening mentalizing (lower scores) was strongly associated with an increase in the severity of depressive symptoms (β = − 0.63 (− 0.53 to − 0.72), p < 0.001) as well as with BPD symptoms (β = − 0.71 (− 0.62 to − 0.78), p < 0.001). The number of BPD symptoms was also strongly associated with the severity of depressive symptoms (β = 0.76 (0.68 to 0.84), p < 0.001).

Multivariate associations

Following adjustment for age, gender, household income, ethnicity, years in education, current employment and the number of BPD symptoms, impairment in mentalizing remained significantly associated with the severity of depressive symptoms but the association was substantially attenuated (β = − 0.18 (− 0.07 to − 0.29), p = 0.002). (Table 2– found at end of file).

Table 2 Linear regression model displaying the association between mentalisation and depressive symptoms, adjusted for socio-demographic factors and co-morbid BPD symptoms

Effect modification of the association between mentalization and depression, by number of BPD symptoms

Likelihood ratio testing revealed that there was a significant interaction between mentalizing impairment and BPD symptoms in the model of depressive symptoms (χ2 = 8.79, p = 0.003). Higher levels of BPD symptoms increased the strength of the association between mentalisation and depressive symptoms (see Supplementary Table 1 for output from the multivariable model including the interaction term).

Figure 1 displays the association between mentalizing and depressive symptoms at 3 key cut-points on the PAIS-BOR: 22.5 (the mean PAIS-BOR score in healthy controls in this sample) as well as at pre-specified cut points of 38 (subsyndromal borderline traits) and 60 (features of borderline personality disorder). (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

The association between mentalization and depression, stratified by levels of Borderline Personality disorder symptoms. The association between mentalizing and depressive symptoms at 3 key cut-points on the PAIS-BOR. Worsening mentalization is more strongly associated with higher depression scores at higher levels of borderline symptomology

Discussion

In this cross-sectional clinical sample, impairment in mentalizing was significantly associated with depressive symptoms. The association was maintained after adjusting for the effects of socio-demographic confounders as well as adjustment for the presence of BPD symptoms, although adjusting for the latter attenuated the strength of the association. This finding is consistent with previous studies suggesting the existence of an independent link between depression and impairment in mentalizing but suggests that a large proportion of the unadjusted relationship is accounted for by covariance with BPD symptoms [6, 9, 13, 14]. To our knowledge, no prior study of this link has accounted for the potential effects of BPD [13,14,15]. It is also worth acknowledging that depression is a heterogenous entity and it may be that mentalizing deficits contribute to the aetiology of certain subtypes of depression more than others. For example, mentalization deficits may play a more substantial role in the development of anaclitic depression among individuals prone to rejection sensitivity, as opposed to the development of introjective depression among those more prone to self-criticism [8]. This potentially warrants exploring in future studies.

Furthermore, we found that the association between depression and mentalisation was moderated by the severity of BPD symptoms; the association between poorer mentalizing and depression was strongest among those with high levels of BPD symptoms.

Given the cross-sectional nature of this study, the findings need to be interpreted cautiously. While our observed findings help support the psychological theory that mentalization deficits contribute to vulnerability to depression among people with BPD, it is plausible that depression reduces the ability to mentalize, as part of a wider range of effects on mental state [13]. These effects may be more pronounced in those with BPD who have pre-existing mentalisation deficits. Another possible explanation is that impairment in mentalization acts as an independent vulnerability factor for depression, with this effect more pronounced in individuals with co-morbid BPD symptoms.

Our findings require replication and further examination in a longitudinal study to establish the direction of association. If mentalization deficits precede the onset of depression, this raises the prospect of mentalization-based treatment (MBT) [22] – an evidence-based treatment for BPD – being applied as a potential prevention strategy for depression.

Our study has several strengths. The size of our sample is considerably larger than previous studies in the field [13, 14] allowing us to examine interaction effects with greater precision. Furthermore, our use of symptom measures, as opposed to categorically defined diagnostic categories allowed us to investigate the links between depressive symptoms and mentalizing with adequate statistical power. Patient samples were recruited from busy NHS settings adding to the generalisability of findings. The findings however need to be considered in the context of certain methodological limitations. The cross-sectional nature of the data means limits our ability to make inferences about temporal sequence underlying the detected associations. Furthermore, the use of a non-random sampling may have introduced selection bias and our use of self-report questionnaires may have introduced reporting bias [23]. Against this, we used validated and reliable scales to reduce measurement error.

Conclusions

Mentalisation impairment is independently associated with increased levels of depressive symptoms. BPD symptoms appear to moderate this association which is more pronounced in the presence of greater levels of BPD symptomatology. The direction and further implications of these associations need to be clarified in a longitudinal study.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BPD:

Borderline Personality Disorder

MBT:

mentalization-based treatment

MZQ:

Mentalisation Questionnaire

BDI-II:

Becks depression inventory -II

PAI-BOR:

Personality Assessment Inventory – Borderline sub-section

MDD:

Major Depressive Disorder

References

  1. Swenson CR, Choi-Kain LW. Mentalization and dialectical behavior therapy. Am J Psychother. 2015;69(2):199–217. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.2015.69.2.199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Midgley N, Vrouva I. Minding the child : mentalization-based interventions with children, young people, and their families. New York: Routledge; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Bourvis N, Aouidad A, Cabelguen C, Cohen D, Xavier J. How do stress exposure and stress regulation relate to borderline personality disorder? Front Psychol. 2017;8:2054. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02054.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Moulton-Perkins A, Lee YW, Warren F, Howard S, et al. Development and validation of a self-report measure of Mentalizing: the reflective functioning questionnaire. PLoS One. 2016;11(7):e0158678. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158678.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Hausberg MC, Schulz H, Piegler T, Happach CG, Klopper M, Brutt AL, et al. Is a self-rated instrument appropriate to assess mentalization in patients with mental disorders? Development and first validation of the mentalization questionnaire (MZQ). Psychother Res. 2012;22(6):699–709. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2012.709325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Allen JG, Fonagy P, Bateman A. Mentalizing in clinical practice. 1st ed. Washington: American Psychiatric Pub; 2008. xix, p. 403.

  7. Herpertz SC, Bertsch K. The social-cognitive basis of personality disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2014;27(1):73–7. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ayduk O, Gyurak A. Applying the cognitive-affective processing systems approach to conceptualizing rejection sensitivity. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2008;2(5):2016–33. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00143.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Choi-Kain LW, Gunderson JG. Borderline personality and mood disorders : comorbidity and controversy. New York: Springer; 2015. xv, p. 278.

  10. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, DeLuca CJ, Hennen J, Khera GS, Gunderson JG. The pain of being borderline: dysphoric states specific to borderline personality disorder. Harv Rev Psychiatr. 1998;6(4):201–7. https://doi.org/10.3109/10673229809000330.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E, Campbell C. What we have changed our minds about: Part 2. Borderline personality disorder, epistemic trust and the developmental significance of social communication. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2017;4:9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Luyten P, Fonagy P. The stress-reward-mentalizing model of depression: an integrative developmental cascade approach to child and adolescent depressive disorder based on the research domain criteria (RDoC) approach. Clin Psychol Rev. 2018;64:87–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Fischer-Kern M, Fonagy P, Kapusta ND, Luyten P, Boss S, Naderer A, et al. Mentalizing in female inpatients with major depressive disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2013;201(3):202–7. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182845c0a.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Fischer-Kern M, Tmej A, Kapusta ND, Naderer A, Leithner-Dziubas K, Loffler-Stastka H, et al. The capacity for mentalization in depressive patients: a pilot study. Z Psychosom Med Psychother. 2008;54(4):368–80. https://doi.org/10.13109/zptm.2008.54.4.368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Taubner S, Kessler H, Buchheim A, Kachele H, Staun L. The role of mentalization in the psychoanalytic treatment of chronic depression. Psychiatry. 2011;74(1):49–57. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.2011.74.1.49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gunderson JG, Morey LC, Stout RL, Skodol AE, Shea MT, McGlashan TH, et al. Major depressive disorder and borderline personality disorder revisited: longitudinal interactions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65(8):1049–56. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v65n0804.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Dubo ED, Sickel AE, Trikha A, Levin A, et al. Axis I comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(12):1733–9. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.155.12.1733.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Beck ATSR, Brown GK. BDI-II, Beck depression inventory : manual. 2nd ed. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Morey LC. Personality assessment inventory (PAI) : professional manual. 2nd ed. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2007. xiii, p. 385.

  20. De Moor MH, Distel MA, Trull TJ, Boomsma DI. Assessment of borderline personality features in population samples: is the personality assessment inventory-borderline features scale measurement invariant across sex and age? Psychol Assess. 2009;21(1):125–30. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014502.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 15.1. College Station: StataCorp LP; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Bateman A, Fonagy P. Randomized controlled trial of outpatient mentalization-based treatment versus structured clinical management for borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(12):1355–64. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Kramer U. Personality, personality disorders, and the process of change. Psychother Res. 2019;29(3):324–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2017.1377358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

N/A

Funding

T. N. is supported by a Wellcome Trust Research Fellowship to PRM which also funded this study. Peter Fonagy is in receipt of a National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Senior Investigator Award (NF-SI-0514 -10157). Peter Fonagy was in part supported by the NIHR Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC) North Thames at Barts Health NHS Trust. BK-C is funded by the National Institutes of Health Award # MH115221. PM is part-funded by the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre at University Hospitals Bristol NHS Foundation Trust and the University of Bristol and is also part-funded by NIHR Applied Research Collaboration (ARC) West. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the UK Department of Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Contributions

RRZ analysed the dataset and wrote the manuscript. PM advised on analysis of the dataset and edited the manuscript, TN and PF were involved in the collection of the dataset and edited the manuscript., JF, RPM, JF, BKC were involved in the collection of the dataset. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to R. P. Rifkin-Zybutz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval was gained for the collection and use of all participants’ data (Research Ethics Committee Wales, 12/WA/0283).

Consent for publication

N/A

Competing interests

The authors report no conflicts of interest

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplementary Table 1

. Linear regression model displaying the association between mentalisation and depressive symptoms with a fitted interaction term with borderline symptoms, adjusted for socio-demographic factors.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rifkin-Zybutz, R.P., Moran, P., Nolte, T. et al. Impaired mentalizing in depression and the effects of borderline personality disorder on this relationship. bord personal disord emot dysregul 8, 15 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-021-00153-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-021-00153-x

Keywords