Skip to main content

A comprehensive evaluation of emotional responsiveness in borderline personality disorder: a support for hypersensitivity hypothesis

Abstract

Background

Many experimental studies have evaluated Linehan’s biological emotional vulnerability in Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD). However, some inconsistencies were observed in operationalizing and supporting its components. This study aims at clarifying which aspects of Linehan’s model are altered in BPD, considering a multimodal evaluation of processes concerned with emotional responsiveness (self-report, psychophysiology and eye-tracking).

Methods

Forty-eight socio-emotional pictures were administered to 28 participants (14 BPD, 14 Healthy Controls, HCs), gender- and age-matched, by employing two different lengths of stimuli exposure (5 s and 15 s).

Results

Our results supported the hypersensitivity hypothesis in terms of faster physiological responses and altered visual processing. Furthermore, hypersensitivity was associated with detailed socio-emotional contents. Hyperreactivity assumption was not experimentally sustained by physiological and self-report data. Ultimately, the slow return to emotional baseline was demonstrated as an impaired emotional modulation.

Conclusions

Our data alternatively supported the hypersensitivity and the slow return to emotional baseline hypotheses, postulated by Linehan’s Biosocial model, rather than the hyperreactivity assumption. Results have been discussed in light of other BPD core psychopathological processes.

Background

Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is a severe mental disorder characterized by a pervasive pattern of instability in affect regulation, impulse control, interpersonal relationships, and self-image [1]. One of the most influential theories of BPD development is Linehan’s Biosocial model [2] that posits emotional dysregulation as the core feature of such disorder. According to this model, emotional dysregulation emerges from continuous transactions between a biological emotional vulnerability and invalidating environments [3, 4]. Specifically, biological vulnerability had been conceptualized considering three different, albeit interrelated, dimensions of emotional functioning: a) heightened emotional sensitivity (e.g., low threshold for emotional reactions); b) intense emotional responses; c) slow return to emotional baseline (e.g., long-lasting emotional reactions). Given the clinical relevance of Linehan’s model for BPD treatment, several experimental studies aimed at empirically testing its dimensions, especially referring to the biological vulnerability. Despite the increasing interest on such topic, empirical findings are controversial and the operationalization of constructs largely varies across studies [5, 6].

Hypersensitivity was originally conceptualized as the tendency to pick up emotional cues (e.g., negative cues), to react quickly, and to have a low threshold for emotional reactions [2]. From an experimental perspective, the hypersensitivity hypothesis was investigated using several physiological indexes and cognitive tasks. Many authors operationalized the low threshold for emotional reactions by assuming a basal physiological hyperarousal, considering different physiological indexes, such as heart rate (HR), skin conductance response (SCR) and level (SCL) and cortisol level (for a review, see: [5]). In addition, Kuo and colleagues [7] specifically referred to basal hyperarousal considering the basal vagal tone according to Porges’s Polyvagal theory [8]. Furthermore, the heightened emotional sensitivity was assessed by evaluating the speed of facial emotion recognition (e.g., [9, 10]), assuming a proneness to rapidly perceive emotional signals in social contexts. Ultimately, emotional hypersensitivity was also investigated postulating a tendency to experience neutral stimuli characterized by an emotional valence [11, 12]. As a whole, it seems that emotional hypersensitivity is ascribed to a general proneness to react. However, no studies investigated whether physiological systems are implicated in such predisposition to emotionally react. Accordingly, an earlier onset of physiological response in relation to the presentation of emotional cues might be considered a reliable index of BPD emotional hypersensitivity. In addition, emotional hypersensitivity might be captured by attentional mechanisms. For instance, this construct was conceptualized as a condition of hypervigilance towards social stimuli [13]. Further operationalization included a faster orientation towards negative emotional faces. A meta-analysis conducted by Kaiser and colleagues [14] partially supported Linehan’s hypersensitivity hypothesis by revealing negative attentional bias for BPD schema congruent words and moderate attentional bias for positive emotional faces. Moreover, a recent work from Kaiser and colleagues [15] extended the previous findings [14] showing that BPD attentional bias was enhanced for threating information. Accordingly, BPD patients seemed mostly characterized by difficulties in disengaging attention from threating cues, rather than an initial allocation towards emotional stimuli, in line with other studies [16, 17]. Despite these results were in line with the concept of hypersensitivity, the data acquisition procedures limited to draw conclusion about the role of attentional processes in operationalizing such dimension of Linehan’s Biosocial model. Indeed, all studies mentioned above considered the manual reaction time and the error rates as outcomes of attentional dysfunctions. However, the use of these measures was called into question in studying attentional processes because they seem to evaluate late and cognitive controlled stages of attentional allocation [18]. Therefore, they do not allow to capture automatic tendencies (e.g., approach-and-avoidance) of attentional systems towards emotional cues [19]. According to the necessity to include reliable measures of attentional mechanisms, the direct estimation of visual processing patterns (e.g., eye-tracking data) represents one of the most common experimental methodologies to assess the attentional allocation on emotional cues [20,21,22]. Particularly, eye movements are an early, cognitively less controlled measure of attentional orienting and a biological marker of immediate shifts in visual attention [18]. Despite the wide use of eye-tracking methodology among several clinical conditions (e.g., [23, 24]) and nonclinical population [25,26,27], solely three studies [19, 28, 29] investigated the automatic attentional allocation on socio-emotional cues among BPD subjects. Particularly, these studies showed that BPD patients exhibited faster saccades towards the eyes of briefly presented (i.e., 150 ms) neutral faces and slower saccades away from fearful eyes [19]. Furthermore, the analysis of eye movements suggested that BPD individuals were characterized by more and faster initial fixation changes to the eyes of angry faces [28]. Eventually, Kaiser and colleagues [29] showed that BPD patients with Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) exhibited more and longer fixations on the eye region of angry/happy blends (ambiguous blends of facial expressions), as well as more fixations on the eye region displaying high levels of sadness compared to HCs. Bertsch and colleagues [19, 28] provided a post-hoc discussion which partially linked the evidences discussed above with Linehan’s Biosocial model. However, these results seem to marginally capture the complexity of visual processing patterns over the course of time. Indeed, according to the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis (e.g., [30]) and the eye-tracking data revealed in other clinical and non-clinical populations (e.g., [31,32,33]), it is possible that after an initial preference for emotional cues, BPD subjects might avoid the same cues in the later stages of visual processing. Nonetheless, no studies investigated attentional allocation patterns among BPD individuals during prolonged exposures (e.g., 5 s) to emotional stimuli in order to clarify whether emotional hypersensitivity could be reflected in visual processing mechanisms. Moreover, previous eye-tracking studies [19, 28, 29] used simple facial stimuli or ambiguous blends. To our knowledge no eye-tracking study was published on BPD sample using more complex visual stimuli.

Marsha Linehan described BPD hyperreactivity in terms of extreme reactions towards emotional stimuli [2]. BPD emotional hyperreactivity has been consistently operationalized within experimental contexts as a heightened stimulus-related change in emotional intensity (e.g., [34,35,36]). On the other hand, methods for assessing such aspect of Biosocial model largely varied from subjective ratings to physiological (e.g., HR, Heart Rate Variability [HRV], SCR and SCL) and behavioral (e.g., electromyography) indexes (for a review see: [37]). Given the huge amount of data concerning the experimental evaluation about this topic, a meta-analytic review was conducted [6] demonstrating that emotional hyperreactivity hypothesis was not supported with regard to several biological indexes. On the contrary, subjective ratings results showed a slightly heightened arousal in response to different emotional stimuli, as well as a tendency to negatively appraise emotional cues. Therefore, BPD emotional hyperreactivity should be mainly related to subjective experiences rather than to altered physiological responses.

The original conceptualization of Biosocial model posited the slow return to emotional baseline in terms of long-lasting physiological reactions to emotional stimuli [2]. The slow return to emotional baseline hypothesis was explicitly examined in few studies [38,39,40], which consistently operationalized and supported this dimension as a delayed habituation of physiological responses after the repetitive presentation of emotional cues. According to Linehan’s model [2] and its extension (i.e., Emotional Cascade Model (e.g., [41])), BPD long-lasting emotional reactions might also reflect the well-documented difficulties in using flexible emotion regulation strategies (e.g., rumination, thought suppression) across different situations [42]. This aspect could be manifested in altered HRV, given its association to a flexible modulation of physiological systems across different conditions [43, 44]. Accordingly, HRV might represent a further valid outcome for the slow return to emotional baseline hypothesis as conceptualized within the Biosocial framework. BPD patients could manifest altered HRV in response to emotional situations, as a manifestation of their inflexible emotional regulation. In addition, given their well-established impairment in habituation [38,39,40], this condition could be particularly manifested when they deal with prolonged emotional situations. As a consequence, a further biological conceptualization of the slow return to emotional baseline should include the evaluation of the modulation mechanisms of physiological systems in response to long-lasting emotional situation.

With regard to the three biological components, the social context seems particularly problematic for BPD patients [45]. The relevance of social stimuli for those with BPD was repeatedly confirmed by previous studies (e.g., [13, 46]). For instance, BPD patients manifested impaired emotional recognition for complex social emotional stimuli, whereas they did not show difficulties in recognizing isolated facial emotional information [46]. In addition, other authors referred to an enhanced social sensitivity reflected by increased vigilance for social stimuli, especially for cues that signaled social rejection or threat (e.g., [13]). Eventually, Weinberg and colleagues [40] showed a specific hyperreactivity to social stressor tasks, whereas no support for hyperreactivity was found considering standard stressor tasks [6].

Taking into account the previous inconsistencies in the operationalization of Linehan’s Biosocial model dimensions and considering the inconclusive empirical findings related to BPD emotional responsiveness, this study aimed at experimentally investigating the emotional hypersensitivity and hyperreactivity hypotheses and the slow return to emotional baseline assumption. Self-report, physiological and eye-tracking data were collected to test the dimensions mentioned above. In line with conceptual considerations discussed previously, the Biosocial dimensions were operationalized considering different indexes of emotional functioning as following:

  1. a)

    With regard to physiological markers, it was assumed that BPD emotional hypersensitivity was effectively conceptualized by an earlier onset of skin conductance response (reduced latency of skin conductance response [SCR Latency]) or/and by heightened basal arousal (increased HR, SCR and SCL; reduced Root Mean Square of the Successive Differences [RMSSD]). Referring to eye-tracking data, it was hypothesized that BPD hypersensitivity could influence both spatial and temporal dimensions of visual processing patterns of emotional cues. In particular, considering a prolonged exposure to complex emotional stimuli, the current study assumed that BPD individuals might exhibit an overall avoidance of socio-emotional contents in terms of reduced spatial and temporal visual exploration (reduced gazes and time spent in Areas of Interest [AoIs]).

  2. b)

    Emotional hyperreactivity was operationalized by enhanced stimulus-related changes in emotional intensity, considering both physiological and self-report indexes. According to meta-analytic results [6], no significant differences would be expected between BPD subjects and healthy controls (HCs) considering physiological indexes (HR, SCR, SCL, RMSSD). On the contrary, a higher self-report arousal might be observed among BPD individuals.

  3. c)

    The slow return to emotional baseline was conceptualized as a difficulty in adapting emotional responses to prolonged emotional situations. Specifically, physiological and self-report results were compared between two experimental conditions, which differed from each other in terms of exposition time to emotional stimuli (i.e., 5 s and 15 s). This choice was supported by studies reporting significant effects of stimuli exposition time on processing mechanism among BPD patients [47, 48]. In line with Fenske and colleagues’ findings [48], it was postulated that BPD subjects might show increasing difficulties in emotion modulation, both considering physiological (RMSSD) and self-report indexes (arousal, valence and dominance) during prolonged exposure to emotional stimuli.

Ultimately, given the well-established difficulties of BPD individuals with social interaction (e.g., [45]), this study employed complex daily-life social pictures in order to improve the naturalistic approach of the experiment. Furthermore, two different types of AoIs were used to analyze eye-tracking data. Particularly, it was taken into consideration either AoI strictly and explicitly connected to emotion expression in social contexts, either larger AoI which included other features in addition to the previous elements. This choice was proposed with the aim of demonstrating that attentional mechanisms associated to BPD emotional hypersensitivity should be specifically related to socio-emotional cues.

Methods

Participants

BPD patients

Fourteen BPD outpatients were included from the Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy Unit of San-Raffaele Hospital (Milan) from January to April 2017. This sample was composed by 11 female and 3 male subjects (mean age = 28.42, SD = 7.43). With regard to educational background, 10 patients (71.4%) had high school degree, 2 (14.3%) had a bachelor degree, and 2 (14.3%) had higher University degree (M.Sc.). Average years of education was 14.00 (SD = 1.71). Clinical subjects met a BPD diagnosis according to DSM-IV criteria assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV axis II Personality Disorders, Version 2.0 (SCID-II) [49]. SCID-II was conducted during the routine diagnostic assessment by trained raters, who were blinded to the hypotheses of this study. The number of BPD traits ranged from 5 to 9 (M = 6.17, SD = 1.75). Exclusion criteria were represented by IQ lower than 70, psychotic disorders and other active psychiatric symptomatology for at least 1 month before task administration (e.g. major depressive episode, current substance use, panic attacks). Lifetime co-diagnoses of other psychiatric disorders did not represent exclusion criteria from the study. Expert psychiatrists conducted clinical interviews for evaluating the presence of exclusion criteria. The mean number of Personality Disorders (PDs) diagnoses was 1.53 (SD = 0.52, range 1–2). PD co-diagnoses were Narcissistic PD (N = 2, 14.3%), Histrionic PD (N = 2, 14.3%), Passive-Aggressive PD (N = 3, 21.4%) and Depressive PD (N = 1, 7.14%). Eight patients (57.14%) did not report any psychiatric comorbidity. Furthermore, 4 patients presented a lifetime psychiatric disorder in comorbidity, namely Major Depressive Disorder (N = 1, 7.14%), Eating Disorder (N = 2, 14.29%), and Substance Use Disorders (N = 1, 7.14%). Pharmacological treatments did not represent exclusion criteria from the study, according to meta-analytic results [6] that showed no relevant confounding effects of this clinical aspect on BPD physiological responsiveness. Thirteen patients took stable pharmacological treatments for at least 3 months. The number of medical prescriptions ranged from 2 to 3. The most commonly prescribed medications were SSRIs (N = 5, 35.71%), anticonvulsant (N = 4, 28.57%), antiepileptics (N = 4, 28.57) and neuroleptics (typical and atypical, N = 4; 14,28%), benzodiazepines (N = 7, 50.0%) and antidepressant (N = 3, 21.43%). However, patients were asked to refrain from taking benzodiazepines 48 h before the experiment.

Healthy controls

Fourteen community dwelling volunteers with negative medical history for psychiatric or neurological disorders were included in the nonclinical sample. HCs were 10 female and 4 male subjects (mean age = 27.57, SD = 5.9). With regard to educational background, 7 participants (50.0%) had high school degree, 5 (35.7%) had a bachelor degree, and 2 (14.3%) had higher University degree (M.Sc.). Mean educational level was 14.79 years (S.D. = 1.97). No significant differences in educational level were found comparing BPD and HC groups (U = 76.50, ns). Participants were preliminarily screened for investigating the presence of certificated psychological, psychiatric and neurological diagnoses, as well as their related treatments. Additional exclusion criteria were IQ lower than 70, current substance use, psychopharmacological treatments and current or lifetime psychological treatments. Ultimately, the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5) [50] and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) [51] were administered in order to exclude the presence of maladaptive personality features and emotional difficulties.

Instruments

Pictures

Forty-eight pictures from the Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS) [52] were administered during the experiment. One-hundred and sixty-one stimuli were selected by the authors of the study from the entire NAPS database including pictures which clearly represented human emotional expressions and social interactions within daily life contexts. The set was administered to 120 community dwelling volunteers who rated each picture on two continuous bipolar semantic sliding scales (arousal and valence) each ranging from 1 to 9, following the procedure of the original database validation [52]. Pictures with mean valence ratings less than 4 were classified as negative; those between 4 and 6 were classified as neutral, and those with mean valence ratings higher than 6 were classified as positive. Sixteen pictures for each valence category (positive, negative, neutral) were selected considering the stimuli that were the most representative for that category. Mean valence was 7.31 (SD = .73) for positive picture, 2.64 (SD = .77) for negative pictures and 4.56 (SD = .62) for neutral pictures.

Pictures were divided in two blocks with different presentation lengths: long (15 s) and short (5 s). Five-seconds picture presentation corresponds to the guidelines for EDA data processing [53]. Moreover, the 15 s exposure was chosen to support a reliable assessment of HRV indexes [54]. No significant differences between blocks were found considering valence and arousal ratings for positive (valence: t(14) = 1.10, ns; arousal: t(14) = − 1.29, ns), negative (valence: t(14) = − 1.29, ns; arousal: t(14) = .23, ns) and neutral pictures (valence: t(14) = −.86, ns; arousal: t(14) = .83, ns). Inter-trial intervals between pictures varied from 10 to 15 s. The order of the stimuli presentation was randomized in order to avoid possible carry-over effects. Selected pictures are listed in Table 1.

Table 1 List of selected pictures

Physiological data

EDA and ECG data were collected by using BITalino [55, 56]. BITalino is a biomedical data acquisition device with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. EDA was collected by the employment of two electrodes on the left palm. EDA results were examined through Ledalab (www.ledalab.de). A 2 Hz low-pass filter was employed for pre-processing the data [57]. Moreover, a Continuous Decomposition Analysis [58] was applied and the principal skin conductance indexes were detached in a 5 s response window after the presentation of each picture. Tonic (SCL), Phasic (SCR) Skin Conductance Response, as well as SCR Latency were calculated from raw physiological data. To normalize the data, a log transformation was used. ECG data was processed by using Kubios (https://www.kubios.com/). Artifact correction and smoothness priors methodology were applied. Heart rate (HR) and the square root of the mean squared differences of successive NN intervals (RMSSD) were calculated from raw ECG data. SCL, SCR and HR were consistent with the literature regarding emotional reactivity evaluation, while RMSSD was considered as an effective and reliable index of physiological adaptation. Lastly, SCR Latency was calculated as an operationalization of Linehan’s physiological hypersensitivity.

Eye-tracking data

Eye-tracking data were collected by using The Eye-Tribe (www.theeyetribe.com), and numerous temporal and spatial outcomes were extracted with Python 2.7. Two types of Areas of Interest (AoI) were designed. First of all, experimental AoIs (AoIs) were a priori defined by the authors, selecting the part/s of the picture which explicitly contained relevant social contents (e.g., emotional faces, social interactions). On the contrary, pilot AoIs (AoIp) were defined by a preliminary study on a non-clinical sample (N = 12) including the portion of the picture mostly looked by the participants on the base of the application of a Gaussian filter on the data. Ultimately, AoIs and AoIp shared the same part of the picture although they varied in their size. In detail, AoIp contained other elements in addition to explicit social contents (e.g., hands, legs, whole body). Prop gaze (i.e., the ratio of the participant’s number of gazes in AoI and the total number of gazes for the picture), mean time (i.e., the participant’s mean time spent in AoI), time 1st fixation (i.e., the amount of time required for reaching the first AoI) and 1st fixation duration (i.e., the amount of time spent in the first looked AoI) were calculated for each picture for AoIs and AoIp. When the picture enclosed more than one AoI, we summed the eyes values for each AoI, with the exclusion of mean time for which the mean values were calculated. Each visual index was corrected by the size of the AoI, excluding time 1st fixation which was corrected by the distance among AoI and the position of the first gaze. Finally, delta scores were calculated for each visual outcome subtracting AoIs from AoIp values, for effectively comparing the visual indexes in the previous areas. Positive scores indicated a predominant orientation of attention on the elements of AoIp that were not included in AoIs. Conversely, negative scores indicated a main focus on detailed socio-emotional contents.

Self-report

Arousal, valence and dominance were rated on a continuous scale ranging from 0 to 1 using three digital sliders (Affective Sliders, AS) [59]. In agreement with the authors of the AS, the dominance slider was added to the two original scales (arousal and valence). The dominance dimension indicated changes in emotional control in response to a specific emotional cue. Lower scores denoted higher control over emotional activation, whereas higher scores represented the feelings of being overwhelmed by the emotional activation elicited by the picture. The poles of the AS (Aroused/Relaxed; Positive/Negative; Dominant/Overwhelmed) were categorized with the presence of an emoticon (i.e., symbolic and stylized facial expression) to give a visual representation of the affective poles of the scales.

Positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) [60]

The PANAS is a 20-item questionnaire developed for assessing the current positive (Positive Affect, PA) and negative (Negative Affect, NA) affectivity. The PANAS is constituted by 10 positive and 10 negative adjectives. Subjects were asked to rate on a 5-point Likert scale (from 1= “very slightly or not at all” to 5 = “extremely”) how much they felt as indicated by the 20 adjectives (e.g., active, determined, excited, nervous, scared, distressed, etc.). The sum of the 10 positive and negative adjectives was calculated to provide a total score for PA and NA, respectively. The original validation showed that the two scales were highly internally consistent, largely uncorrelated, and stable at appropriate levels over a 2-month time period [60]. In our sample, we administered the Italian version of the scale [61]. The PANAS factor structure and solid psychometric proprieties were also confirmed for the Italian version [61].

Difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS) [51]

The DERS is a 36-items multidimensional questionnaire for evaluating six emotion dysregulation scales (Nonacceptance of Emotional Responses; Difficulties Engaging in Goal-Directed Behavior; Impulse Control Difficulties; Lack of Emotional Awareness; Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies; and Lack of Emotional Clarity). Participants were asked to rate the 36 items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“almost never”) to 5 (“almost always”). DERS total score was given by the sum of the 36 items. Moreover, scale scores were calculated for the six subscales. Robust psychometric proprieties were reported for the original version with an internal consistency of .93, a test-retest reliability of .88 during a 4-week to 8-week interval, and a clear factor structure. Additionally, good construct validity and a high internal consistency was found in clinical and nonclinical populations [51]. The Italian version of the instrument was administered in our sample [62]. The DERS factorial structure, good internal consistency and test-retest reliability were replicated for the Italian version [62].

Personality inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5) [50]

The PID-5 is a 220-item questionnaire evaluating DSM-5 maladaptive personality traits and domains. The 220 items were rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 0 (“very false or often false”) to 3 (“very true or often true”). The PID-5 has 25 primary scales that load onto 5 higher order dimensions (Negative affectivity, Detachment, Disinhibition, Antagonism and Psychoticism). As a result of differences in scale length, mean scale scores were used instead of sum, both for the 5 domains and the 25 subscales. Adequate internal consistency was found for PID-5 traits and domains [50]. In our sample, the Italian version of the questionnaire was administered [63]. Robust psychometric proprieties were confirmed also for clinical and nonclinical Italian samples, both with regard to internal consistency and factorial structure [63].

Procedure

The complete process was carried out in laboratory setting at San-Raffaele Hospital, Milan from 11.00 a.m. to 2.00 p.m. Participants were asked to refrain from drinking coffee 2 h before the experiment or smoking cigarettes 1 h before the experiment. Additionally, alcohol or illicit drugs used 24 h before the experiment represented an exclusion criterion. Informed consent was signed prior to the experiment. Participants completed the PANAS and other pre-task questionnaires (e.g., additional clinical and medical information). A 2 min baseline for physiological parameters was recorded. Then, participants were asked to complete a small practice part of 5 pictures. The experiment was composed by two blocks of 24 pictures each (long and short setup), with a small break between them. The order of the stimuli presentation was randomized for each participant, as well as the order of the blocks. Before each picture, a fixation cross was displayed for 1 s on the screen and participants were instructed to look at the cross until the picture appeared. Pictures were presented for 5 s or 15 s, accordingly to the block. Subsequently, three rating scales (Arousal, Valence and Dominance) were presented on the screen. Subjects were instructed to rate each scale using the mouse and then to move to the next picture by clicking on a “Continue” button on the screen. During the whole procedure, EDA, ECG and eye-tracking data were continuously recorded. After the task, participants completed the PANAS. The DERS and the PID-5 were completed by participants after the experiment, at home.

Statistical analysis

Non-parametric procedures were proposed to analyze data in line with the small sample size and the violations of normal distribution for several measures included in the study. Particularly, Mann-Whitney U test was computed to compare BPD and HC subjects considering clinical variables assessing the severity of BPD psychopathology (i.e., DERS and PID-5 scores) and state variables associated to the experimental context (i.e., pre- and post-task PANAS scores). The exact test was used to compute p-values. The r coefficient was calculated as an effect size measure for non-parametric comparisons between groups [64, 65]. The Aligned Rank Transform using ARTool program [66] was applied in order to evaluate non-parametric main effect of group and interaction effects group by valence of emotional stimuli (i.e., negative, positive, neutral; group x category) and block (i.e., short and long; group x block), taking into account indexes of emotional functioning measured during the task (i.e., self-report, physiological and eye-tracking data). The aligned transformation refers to a preprocessing procedure that aligns the data for each effect (main or interaction) before assigning ranks averaged in the case of ties. Data alignment is an established process in statistics [67] by which effects are estimated as marginal means and then “stripped” from the response variable so that all effects but one are removed. After the aligned rank transformation of data for each effect, factorial ANOVA was conducted to evaluate the significance of main and interaction effects, which was estimated using the F-test [66]. Partial η2 (pη2) was utilized as an effect size measure of non-parametric main and interaction effects. Post-hoc comparisons were based on Mann-Whitney U tests using exact test for the estimation of p-values. With regard to post-hoc analyses, adequate Bonferroni’s correction was applied when multiple comparisons were performed. Ultimately, Monte Carlo simulation based on 10,000 independent samples and its 2-tailed 99% confidence interval (CI) was employed in order to evaluate the robustness of between-group comparisons. Spearman’s correlation was used to evaluate associations among different emotional response systems (i.e., self-report scores, physiology and eye-tracking).

Results

Table 2 shows between group comparisons concerning emotional state variables (i.e., PANAS pre- and post-task scores) and questionnaires scores (i.e., DERS and PID-5 scores). As expected, BPD subjects reported significantly higher scores in all PID-5 domains/traits and DERS scales compared to HCs, with the exception of DERS Awareness subscale and PID-5 Antagonism domain and Risk Taking facet. The analyses did not detect significant differences in pre-task PANAS scores. Therefore, it was possible to exclude confounding effects of pre-existing emotional state on responsiveness to experimental paradigm. Furthermore, no significant differences were observed in PANAS post-task levels and pre- post-task change scores (PANAS PA: U = 80.50, Z = −.82, ns; PANAS NA: U = 85.50, Z = −.58, ns).

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and non-parametric comparisons related to questionnaires data

Tables 3, 4 and 5 summarize descriptive statistics concerning self-report, eye-tracking and physiological data collected during the task. Table 6 summarizes factorial ANOVA results for self-report, physiological and visual indexes.

Table 3 Descriptive statistics related to self-reported data
Table 4 Descriptive statistics related to eye-tracking data in experimental and pilot Areas of Interest
Table 5 Descriptive statistics related to physiological data
Table 6 Factorial ANOVA results. Group effect and Group x Category and Group x Block interactions for included indexes

Five BPD subjects were omitted from EDA analysis because of technical problems in recording electrodermal activity. However, we compared BPD included and excluded subjects (BPD patients with EDA recording vs. BPD patients without EDA recording) with regard to relevant clinical variables. BPD excluded subjects did not differ from included participants, considering all the clinical variables assessed in the study, namely number of BPD (U = 15.5, ns) and other PDs’ traits (all p < .05), DERS (U = 37.0, ns) and PID-5 scores (Uneg. aff = 9.0, ns; Udetach = 14.0, ns; Uantag = 9.0, ns; Udisin = 15.0, ns; Upsychot = 13.0, ns), number of psychiatric disorders (U = 7.0, ns) and pharmacological treatment (number and type of prescriptions; all p < .05).

Hypersensitivity

The analyses did not reveal between-group differences considering basal physiological responses (SCR, SCL, HR and RMSSD). On the contrary, a significant main effect of group was found for SCR Latency (F(1,21) = 4.39; p < .05, pη2 = .17). Specifically, BPD patients exhibited a faster onset of physiological responses (see Table 5), mostly in relation to the exposure to negative (U = 24.00; Z = − 2.46; p < .0167, Monte Carlo 99% CI: [.009–.015]; r = −.46) and neutral (U = 32.00, Z = − 1.95; p = .05; Monte Carlo 99% CI: [.04–.06]; r = −.36) stimuli. Exclusively the first comparison is significant after Bonferroni correction (α/3 = .0167).

As regard to eye-tracking data, a significant main effect of group was found for prop gaze (F(1,26) = 5.73; p < .05, pη2 = .18). Particularly, BPD patients showed a significant lower number of gazes when exploring socio-emotional cues included in AoIs (see Table 4), especially in relation to stimuli characterized by a positive valence (U = 46.00; Z = − 2.39; p < .0167, Monte Carlo 99% CI: [.01–.016]; r = −.45) (Bonferroni correction: α/3 = .0167). A significant main effect of group was also observed with respect to 1st fixation duration (F(1,26) = 7.64; p < .05; pη2 = .23). Specifically, BPD subjects spent significantly less time in the first looked AoIs (see Table 4) compared to HCs. Ultimately, the analyses revealed a significant main group effect for the mean time (F(1,26) = 5.40; p < .05; pη2 = .17) spent in AoIs, with BPD patients reporting shorter mean time in AoIs compared to HCs. Overall, BPD patients spent less time in exploring socio-emotional cues as manifested by lower (prop) gaze, shorter 1st fixation duration and shorter mean time compared to HCs (see Table 4). On the contrary, no significant between-group difference was found for time 1st fixation.

Hyperreactivity

The analyses did not find significant main and interaction effects in relation to the size of stimulus-related changes in self-report (i.e., arousal, valence, dominance) and physiological (i.e., SCR, SCL, HR, RMSSD) measures.

Slow return to emotional baseline: difficulties with emotion modulation

A group x block interaction was observed for valence ratings (F(2,25) = 13.54; p < .01; pη2 = .34) (Fig. 1). Particularly, BPD patients reported a reduction of valence ratings when results from the short block (5 s) were compared to the long one (15 s). On the contrary, HCs exhibited an opposite pattern of self-report responses. The difference between these trends was significant (U = 27.50; Z = − 3.24; p < .01; Monte Carlo 99% CI: [.000–.002]; r = −.61) (Bonferroni correction: α/2 = .025). No further interaction effects were detected for the other subjective experience domains. Considering the physiological indexes, the analyses showed a significant group x block interaction for RMSSD (F(2,20) = 10.05; p < .01; pη2 = .29) (Fig. 2). The RMSSD was higher for stimuli presented for 5 s, compared to 15-s presentation condition, among BPD subjects. Conversely, HCs exhibited higher levels of RMSSD when they dealt with stimuli presented for 15 s. The difference between blocks for RMSSD levels was significant comparing BPD subjects with HCs (U = 35.00, Z = -2.89, p < .01; Monte Carlo 99% CI: [.002–.005]; r = −.54) (Bonferroni correction: α/2 = .025).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Group x Block interaction for Valence ratings

Fig. 2
figure 2

Group x Block interaction for RMSSD

The effect of socio-emotional content

When pilot AoI were considered, the significant main group effect was replicated exclusively for 1st fixation duration (F(1,26) = 8.09; p < .01; pη2 = .24). With respect to this eye-tracking index, it was also observed a significant group x category interaction effect (F(2,25) = 3.53; p < .05; pη2 = .22). Specifically, the duration of the 1st fixation was significantly shorter among BPD subjects compared to HCs, when considering stimuli characterized by a positive valence (U = 39.00; Z = − 2.71; p < .0167; Monte Carlo 99% CI: [.002–.006]; r = −.51) (Bonferroni correction: α/3 = .0167).

Computing delta scores (i.e., pilot – experimental) of eye-tracking indexes between AoIp and AoIs, significant differences between groups were detected considering prop gaze (U = 53.00; Z = 2.07; p < .05; Monte Carlo 99% CI: [.015–.022]; r = .39) and mean time (U = 55.00; Z = 1.98; p < .05; Monte Carlo 99% CI: [.020–.028]; r = .37). Particularly, BPD group showed larger differences in visual processing pattern between AoIp and AoIs than HCs.

Congruency among self-report, physiological and eye-tracking measures

No significant correlation was found among self-report, physiological and eye-tracking indexes in BPD patients and in HCs.

Discussion

This study sought to investigate the dimensions that characterize BPD biological emotional vulnerability, as hypothesized by Linehan’s Biosocial model [2]. Particularly, it was adopted a comprehensive assessment approach (i.e., self-report, physiological, eye-tracking) in order to test and to propose an alternative operationalization of the hypersensitivity and the hyperreactivity hypothesis, as well as considering the slow return to emotion baseline assumption. Ultimately, it was also explored how selective socio-emotional cues could influence visual processing patterns of BPD individuals, taking into account the well-established difficulties with social interactions associated to such disorder [45].

Our results partially supported the hypersensitivity hypothesis as demonstrated by an earlier onset of sympathetic response to social cues. This proneness to physiologically react seems to be particularly relevant for stimuli characterized by a negative and neutral valence. According to these findings, the original conceptualization of emotional hypersensitivity, which described it as a reduced threshold for emotional responses, might be explained by a tendency to rapidly react to different kind of emotional cues. On the one hand, considering the effect of negative emotional stimuli on the onset of physiological response, this finding might be in accordance with other studies [68,69,70] demonstrating that BPD subjects showed a specific alteration in physiological responsivity to negative emotional situations. On the other hand, the rapid physiological response associated to the presentation of stimuli characterized by a neutral valence might reflect the well-documented tendency of BPD subjects to negatively interpret neutral cues (for a review see: [12]). Furthermore, the hypersensitivity assumption seems to be supported by altered visual processing mechanisms in exploring emotional stimuli. As expected, spatial and temporal eye-tracking indexes suggested that BPD subjects are characterized by a reduced visual exploration of socio-emotional cues. This finding partially replicated Bertsch and colleagues’ results [28] showing that BPD subjects exhibited alterations in visual exploration when dealing with selective emotional cues (e.g., eyes). Furthermore, according to the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis (e.g., [30]) and other eye-tracking studies (e.g., [31,32,33]), BPD subjects might massively use attentional avoidance strategies [71], especially when they deal with explicit social-emotional contents. This aspect of BPD functioning could be ascribed to the well-established association to rejection sensitivity [72] that leads BPD patients to misinterpret social signals as untrustworthy and avoiding them as a consequence [73,74,75]. Interestingly, eye-tracking data revealed a lower number of gazes in AoIs when BPD subjects were exposed to stimuli characterized by a positive valence. This finding could be in line with theoretical and empirical evidence that demonstrated that BPD subjects tend to suppress the positive elements of internal experiences and external situations in the light of cognitive schemas (for a review, see: [76]) that filter incongruent information with self-others dysfunctional representations [77, 78]. Nonetheless, future experimental studies should be carried out in order to validate the hypothesized relationship between rejection sensitivity and dysfunctional cognitive representations with attentional and visual processes.

Considering no significant differences in the amplitude of physiological responses and in self-report levels of emotional reactivity (i.e., arousal, valence, dominance), the hyperreactivity hypothesis was not supported by the current experimental task. This result is not surprising in the light of meta-analytic conclusions [6] that showed consistent and null effect sizes between BPD subjects and HCs, taking into account several physiological indexes of emotional reactivity. On the contrary, different trends between BPD subjects and HCs were observed in self-report and cardiac responses when the length of stimuli presentation was considered. Specifically, BPD subjects rated the longer stimuli (15 s) as more negative than the ones presented for 5 s. Furthermore, the clinical group showed lower levels of RMSSD index in the 15-s exposure condition than in the other condition. Conversely, HCs showed inverse trends considering both indexes. Taken these findings together, it could be possible to conclude that BPD subjects, contrary to HCs, exhibited difficulties in adapting physiological systems implicated in emotion regulation [43, 44, 79, 80] when dealing with prolonged exposure to socio-emotional situations. This kind of physiological alteration might preliminary explain the biological underpinnings that contribute to the conceptualization of the slow return to emotional baseline. Strictly connected to this biological aspect and the results regarding self-report levels of negative valance, the slow return to emotional baseline might be also attributed to the maladaptive effects of the rigid use of emotion regulation strategies (e.g., rumination, experiential avoidance). It was previously demonstrated that maladaptive emotion regulation affects both emotional subjective experience [41, 42] and long-term physiological responses [81,82,83,84]. Eventually, this aspect could be heightened when BPD subjects are exposed to long-lasting emotional situations. Nevertheless, future research should empirically investigate how dysfunctional emotion regulation strategies are involved in explaining the slow return to emotional baseline operationalized as an overall difficulty in adapting physiological systems to contextual variability.

Referring to visual processing mechanisms implicated in defining emotional hypersensitivity, the results supported that specific socio-emotional cues should be considered the core challenging aspects of a situation for BPD subjects. Indeed, the current findings suggested that BPD patients seem to avoid facial expressions and interpersonal cues in favor of an orientation of attention to other elements of the picture. Accordingly, the well-established BPD difficulties in managing social interactions could be ascribed to an incapability to appraise complex socio-emotional signals.

Ultimately, no significant associations were found considering self-report, physiological and eye-tracking responses to socio-emotional pictures in BPD patients and HCs. This finding is in line with Cavazzi and Becerra’s review [5] and with the results of a current meta-analysis by Bortolla, Cavicchioli and colleagues [6] supporting the hypothesis of a substantial decoupling among subjective experience, physiological activation, and behavioral responses in BPD patients. From the perspective of the Emotional Coherence theory [85], different components of emotional response (i.e., subjective response, behavior and physiology) are rarely coherent, and they respond in multiple ways to environmental demands especially in psychopathology. As a consequence, treatments for BPD need to incorporate gradual exposure to specific emotionally provocative stimuli to enhance the emotional coherence between the different emotional systems. However, further research is needed to deepen this topic.

Despite the considerations mentioned above, several limitations need to be discussed. First of all, although an adequate statistical methodology was applied to support the robustness of findings and the number of participants were similar to other psychophysiological studies on BPD emotional responsiveness [35, 85, 86], the sample size represents the primary limitation to the results’ generalizability, especially considering electrodermal data. Therefore, future replication studies administering the current experimental task should be carried out in order to sustain the operationalization of emotional hypersensitivity and the slow return to emotional baseline proposed in the current study. Moreover, the small sample did not allow to control the results for possible confounding effects of gender on emotional responsiveness, as demonstrated in other neuroimaging (e.g., [87]) and psychophysiological (e.g., [88, 89]) studies. Thus, further studies should test BPD gender differences in emotional dysregulation, taking into account several domains of emotional functioning (i.e., self-report, physiological, attentional).

Secondly, although our findings on eye-tracking indexes were in accordance with the hypothesis of altered attentional processes, it is possible that the reduced BPD visual exploration could be explained by secondary elements of the pictures that attract their focus of attention. Thus, these results should be replicated by comparing social cues with non-social pictures (e.g., objects, landscapes). Furthermore, given the high co-occurrence of other psychiatric disorders (e.g., major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders) among BPD subjects and the absence of clinical control groups characterized by overlapped physiological and attentional dysfunctions [90,91,92], it was not possible to definitively ascribed emotional dysfunctions observed in the current study to BPD pathology. As a result, future research investigating the biological emotional vulnerability of Linehan’s Biosocial model should compare BPD subjects with other psychopathological conditions to clarify the core dysfunctional mechanisms associated to the disorder.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this is the first study that evaluated subjective, physiological and visual processing implicated in the comprehensive operationalization of the biological emotional vulnerabilities postulated by the Linehan’s Biosocial model. The results supported the hypersensitivity and the slow return to emotional baseline hypotheses. On the contrary, the hyperreactivity hypothesis was not sustained by empirical data.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.

  2. Linehan MM. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Crowell SE, Beauchaine TP, Linehan MM. A biosocial developmental model of borderline personality: elaborating and extending Linehan’s theory. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(3):495–510.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR. The essential nature of borderline psychopathology. J Personal Disord. 2007;21(5):518–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Cavazzi T, Becerra R. Psychophysiological research of borderline personality disorder: Review and implications for biosocial theory. Eur J Psychol. 2014;10(1):185–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Bortolla R, Cavicchioli M, Fossati A, Maffei C. Emotional reactivity in borderline personality disorder: theoretical considerations based on a meta-analytic review of laboratory studies. J Personal Disord. 2018:1–24. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2018_32_382.

  7. Kuo JR, Linehan MM. Disentangling emotion processes in borderline personality disorder: physiological and self-reported assessment of biological vulnerability, baseline intensity, and reactivity to emotionally evocative stimuli. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009;118(3):531–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Porges SW, Doussard-Roosevelt JA, Portales AL, Greenspan SI. Infant regulation of the vagal “brake” predicts child behavior problems: a psychobiological model of social behavior. Dev Psychobiol. 1996;29(8):697–712.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Domes G, Czieschnek D, Weidler F, Berger C, Fast K, Herpertz SC. Recognition of facial affect in borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 2008;22(2):135–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Lynch TR, Rosenthal MZ, Kosson DS, Cheavens JS, Lejuez CW, Blair RJR. Heightened sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion in borderline personality disorder. Emotion. 2006;6(4):647–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Carpenter RW, Trull TJ. Components of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder: A review. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15(1):335.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Daros AR, Zakzanis KK, Ruocco AC. Facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med. 2013;43(9):1953–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Frick C, Lang S, Kotchoubey B, Sieswerda S, Dinu-Biringer R, Berger M, et al. Hypersensitivity in borderline personality disorder during mindreading. PLoS One. 2012;7(8):4–11.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Kaiser D, Jacob GA, Domes G, Arntz A. Attentional Bias for emotional stimuli in borderline personality disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychopathology. 2017;49(6):383–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Kaiser D, Jacob GA, van Zutphen L, Siep N, Sprenger A, Tuschen-Caffier B, et al. Patients with borderline personality disorder and comorbid PTSD show biased attention for threat in the facial dot-probe task. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.11.005.

  16. Jovev M, Green M, Chanen A, Cotton S, Coltheart M, Jackson H. Attentional processes and responding to affective faces in youth with borderline personality features. Psychiatry Res. 2012;199(1):44–50 Elsevier.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna IA, Brunner R, Parzer P, Mundt C, Fiedler P, Resch F. Initial orienting to emotional faces in female adolescents with borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology. 2010;43(2):79–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Bannerman RL, Milders M, De Gelder B, Sahraie A. Orienting to threat: faster localization of fearful facial expressions and body postures revealed by saccadic eye movements. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2009;276(1662):1635–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Bertsch K, Krauch M, Stopfer K, Haeussler K, Herpertz SC, Gamer M. Interpersonal threat sensitivity in borderline personality disorder: an eye-tracking study. J Personal Disord. 2017;31(5):647–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Pomplun M, Ritter H, Velichkovsky B. Disambiguating complex visual information: towards communication of personal views of a scene. Perception. 1996;25(8):931–48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Rehder B, Hoffman AB. Thirty-something categorization results explained: selective attention, eyetracking, and models of category learning. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2005;31(5):811–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Wooding DS, Mugglestone MD, Purdy KJ, Gale AG. Eye movements of large populations: I. Implementation and performance of an autonomous public eye tracker. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2002;34(4):509–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Chen NTM, Clarke PJF. Gaze-based assessments of vigilance and avoidance in social anxiety: a review. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2017;19(9):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Pinhas L, Fok KH, Chen A, Lam E, Schachter R, Eizenman O, et al. Attentional biases to body shape images in adolescents with anorexia nervosa: an exploratory eye-tracking study. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220(1–2):519–26 Elsevier.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Calvo MG, Nummenmaa L, Avero P. Visual search of emotional faces eye-movement assessment of component processes. Exp Psychol. 2008;55(6):359–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Nummenmaa L, Hyönä J, Calvo MG. Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion. 2006;6(2):257–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Rayner K. Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Q J Exp Psychol. 2009;62:1457–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Bertsch K, Gamer M, Schmidt B, Schmidinger I, Walther S, Kastel T, et al. Oxytocin and reduction of social threat hypersensitivity in women with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(10):1169–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Kaiser D, Jacob GA, van Zutphen L, Siep N, Sprenger A, Tuschen-Caffier B, Senft A, Arntz A, Domes G. Biased attention to facial expression of ambiguous emotions in borderline personality disorder: an eyetracking study. J Personal Disord. 2019;33:1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Mogg K, Bradley BP, De Bono J, Painter M. Time course of attentional bias for threat information in non-clinical anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 1997;35(4):297–303.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Wieser MJ, Pauli P, Weyers P, Alpers GW, Mühlberger A. Fear of negative evaluation and the hypervigilance-avoidance hypothesis: an eye-tracking study. J Neural Transm. 2009;116(6):717–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Gamble AL, Rapee RM. The time-course of attentional bias in anxious children and adolescents. J Anxiety Disord. 2009;23(7):841–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Gibb BE, McGeary JE, Beevers CG. Attentional biases to emotional stimuli: key components of the RDoC constructs of sustained threat and loss. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2016;171(1):65–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Herpertz SC, Kunert HJ, Schwenger UB, Sass H. Affective responsiveness in borderline personality disorder: a psychophysiological approach. Am J Psychiatr. 1999;156(10):1550–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Schmahl CG, Elzinga BM, Ebner UW, Simms T, Sanislow C, Vermetten E, et al. Psychophysiological reactivity to traumatic and abandonment scripts in borderline personality and posttraumatic stress disorders: a preliminary report. Psychiatry Res. 2004;126(1):33–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Suvak MK, Sege CT, Sloan DM, Shea MT, Yen S, Litz BT. Emotional processing in borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord Theory Res Treat. 2012;3(3):273–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Rosenthal MZ, Gratz KL, Kosson DS, Cheavens JS, Lejuez CW, Lynch TR. Borderline personality disorder and emotional responding: a review of the research literature. Clin Psychol Rev. 2008;28(1):75–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Austin MA, Riniolo TC, Porges SW. Borderline personality disorder and emotion regulation: insights from the polyvagal theory. Brain Cogn. 2007;65(1):69–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Dziobek I, Preißler S, Grozdanovic Z, Heuser I, Heekeren HR, Roepke S. Neuronal correlates of altered empathy and social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Neuroimage. 2011;57(2):539–48 Elsevier Inc.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Weinberg A, Klonsky ED, Hajcak G. Autonomic impairment in borderline personality disorder: a laboratory investigation. Brain Cogn. 2009;71(3):279–86 Elsevier Inc.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Selby EA, Joiner TE Jr. Cascades of emotion: the emergence of borderline personality disorder from emotional and behavioral dysregulation. Rev Gen Psychol. 2009;13(3):219–29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Cavicchioli M, Rugi C, Maffei C. Inability to withstand present-moment experiences in borderline personality disorder: a meta-analytic review. Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2015;12(4):101–10.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Rev Gen Psychol. 2006;10(3):229–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord. 2000;61(3):201–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Lis S, Bohus M. Social interaction in borderline personality disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15(2):338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Minzenberg MJ, Poole JH, Vinogradov S. Social-emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):468–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Dyck M, Habel U, Slodczyk J, Schlummer J, Backes V, Schneider F, Reske M. Negative bias in fast emotion discrimination in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med. 2009;39(5):855–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Fenske S, Lis S, Liebke L, Niedtfeld I, Kirsch P, Mier D. Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: effects of emotional information on negative bias. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2015;2(1):10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Maffei C, Fossati A, Barraco A, Bagnato M, Deborah D, Namia C, et al. Interrater reliability and internal consistency of the structured clinical interview for Dsm-iv Axis ii personality. J Personal Disord. 1997;11(3):279–84.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Krueger RF, Derringer J, Markon KE, Watson D, Skodol AE. Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM-5. Psychol Med. 2012;42(9):1879–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26(1):41–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Marchewka A, Zurawski Ł, Jednoróg K, Grabowska A. The Nencki affective picture system (NAPS): introduction to a novel, standardized, wide-range, high-quality, realistic picture database. Behav Res Methods. 2014;46(2):596–610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Braithwaite JJ, Watson DG, Jones R, Rowe M. A guide for analysing electrodermal activity (EDA) & skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychological experiments. Psychophysiology. 2013;49(1):1017–34.

  54. Wu W, Lee J. Improvement of HRV methodology for positive/negative emotion assessment. In: Proc 5th Int ICST Conf Collab Comput Networking, Appl Work; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Guerreiro J, Martins R, Silva H, Lourenço A, Fred A. BITalino: a multimodal platform for physiological computing. In: Proc 10th ICINCO Conf; 2013. p. 500–6.

    Google Scholar 

  56. da Silva HP, Guerreiro J, Lourenço A, Fred AL, Martins R. BITalino: a novel hardware framework for physiological computing. In: PhyCS; 2014. p. 246–53.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Mitra SK, Kuo Y. Digital signal processing. a computer-based approach (Vol. 2). New York: McGraw-Hill; 2006.

  58. Benedek M, Kaernbach C. Decomposition of skin conductance data by means of nonnegative deconvolution. Psychophysiology. 2010;47(4):647–58.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Betella A, Verschure PFMJ. The affective slider: a digital self-assessment scale for the measurement of human emotions. PLoS One. 2016;11(2):1–11.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. PA and NA PANAS: development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Therapy. 1988;54(1988):1995.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Terraciano A, McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. Factorial and construct validity of the Italian positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Eur J Psychol Assess. 2003;19(2):131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Giromini L, Velotti P, De Campora G, Bonalume L, Cesare ZG. Cultural adaptation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale: reliability and validity of an Italian version. J Clin Psychol. 2012;68(9):989–1007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Fossati A, Krueger RF, Markon KE, Borroni S, Maffei C. Reliability and validity of the personality inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5): predicting DSM-IV personality disorders and psychopathy in community-dwelling Italian adults. Assessment. 2013;20(6):689–708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Field A. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. New York: Sage; 2013.

  65. Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic procedures for social research (Vol. 6). Thousand Oaks: Sage; 1991.

  66. Wobbrock JO, Findlater L, Gergle D, Higgins JJ. The aligned rank transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only anova procedures. In: Proc 2011 annu conf hum factors comput syst; 2011. p. 143.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Hodges JL, Lehmann EL. Rank methods for combination of independent experiments in analysis of variance. Ann Math Stat. 1962;33(2):482–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Reichenberger J, Eibl JJ, Pfaltz M, Wilhelm FH, Voderholzer U, Hillert A, et al. Don’t praise me, Don’t chase me: emotional reactivity to positive and negative social-evaluative videos in patients with borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 2017;31(1):75–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Schulze L, Schmahl C, Niedtfeld I. Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: a multimodal meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79(2):97–106 Elsevier.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Stepp SD, Scott LN, Jones NP, Whalen DJ, Hipwell AE. Negative emotional reactivity as a marker of vulnerability in the development of borderline personality disorder symptoms. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;28(1):213–24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Berenson KR, Gyurak A, Downey G, Garner MJ, Mogg K, Bradley BP, et al. Rejection sensitivity and disruption of attention by social threat. J Res Pers. 2009;43(6):1064–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Gao S, Assink M, Cipriani A, Lin K. Associations between rejection sensitivity and mental health outcomes: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;57:59–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Burklund LJ, Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD. The face of rejection: rejection sensitivity moderates dorsal anterior cingulate activity to disapproving facial expressions. Soc Neurosci. 2007;2(3–4):238–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Fertuck EA, Grinband J, Stanley B. Facial trust appraisal negatively biased in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2013;207(3):195–202 Elsevier.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Miano A, Fertuck EA, Arntz A, Stanley B. Rejection sensitivity is a mediator between borderline personality disorder features and facial trust appraisal. J Pers Disord. 2013;27(4):442–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Barazandeh H, Kissane DW, Saeedi N, Gordon M. A systematic review of the relationship between early maladaptive schemas and borderline personality disorder/traits. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;94:130–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Lobbestael J, Arntz A. Cognitive contributions to personality disorders. In: Oxford handbook of personality disorders. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Young JE, Klosko JS, Weishaar ME. Schema therapy: a practitioner’s guide. New York: Guilford Press; 2003.

  79. Beauchaine TP. Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory: toward an integrated model of autonomie nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2001;13(2):183–214.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Fujimura T, Okanoya K. Heart rate variability predicts emotional flexibility in response to positive stimuli. Psychology. 2012;03(08):578–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH, Brown TA, Hofmann SG. Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44(9):1251–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Glynn LM, Christenfeld N, Gerin W. The role of rumination in recovery from reactivity: cardiovascular consequences of emotional states. Psychosom Med. 2002;64(5):714–26.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Sloan DM. Emotion regulation in action: emotional reactivity in experiential avoidance. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(11):1257–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Bichescu-Burian D, Steyer J, Steinert T, Grieb B, Tschöke S. Trauma-related dissociation: psychological features and psychophysiological responses to script-driven imagery in borderline personality disorder. Psychophysiology. 2005;54(3):452–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Mauss IB, Levenson RW, McCarter L, Wilhelm FJ, Gross JJ. The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behaviour and physiology. Emotion. 2005;5(2):175–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Simeon D, Knutelska M, Smith L, Baker BR, Hollander E. A preliminary study of cortisol and norepinephrine reactivity to psychosocial stress in borderline personality disorder with high and low dissociation. Psychiatry Res. 2005;149(1):177–84.

    Google Scholar 

  87. McRae K, Ochsner KN, Mauss IB, Gabrieli JJ, Gross JJ. Gender differences in emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2008;11(2):143–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Labouvie-Vief G, Lumley MA, Jain E, Heinze H. Age and gender differences in cardiac reactivity and subjective emotion responses to emotional autobiographical memories. Emotion. 2003;3(2):115–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Bradley MM, Codispoti M, Sabatinelli D, Lang PJ. Emotion and motivation II: sex differences in picture processing. Emotion. 2001;1(3):300–19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Bylsma LM, Morris BH, Rottenberg J. A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. 2008;28(4):676–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(1):1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Gotlib IH, Krasnoperova E, Yue DN, Joormann J. Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004;113(1):127–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Ms. Elisabetta Masci for her support in English language revision and Mr. Pietro Ramella for his assistance with patients recruitment.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RB, MC and MG participated in the design of the study, carried out the study, performed the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. PV and CM contributed to the theoretical background of the study and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roberta Bortolla.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted in line with the American Psychological Association’s Ethical Standards for Research and Publication. The protocol of this study was submitted to IRCCS San Raffaele hospital ethics committee. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bortolla, R., Cavicchioli, M., Galli, M. et al. A comprehensive evaluation of emotional responsiveness in borderline personality disorder: a support for hypersensitivity hypothesis. bord personal disord emot dysregul 6, 8 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-019-0105-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-019-0105-4

Keywords