Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Low positive affect display mediates the association between borderline personality disorder and negative evaluations at zero acquaintance

Abstract

Background

Several recent studies have demonstrated that naïve raters tend to evaluate individuals with Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) negatively at zero-acquaintance (i.e., in a ‘first impression’ type situation, where the rater has no knowledge of the individual and no prior interactions with them). Specifically, individuals with BPD were evaluated as less trustworthy, likeable, and cooperative than healthy participants (HCs). Based on previous impression formation studies, we hypothesized that the non-verbal cues positive affect display, negative affect display, and eye contact contribute to negative first impressions of those with BPD.

Methods

To address this question, we recruited 101 participants that rated the degree of positive affect display, negative affect display, and eye contact in 52 videos of age-and gender-matched BPD and HC participants. We hypothesized that low positive affect display, high negative affect display, and eye contact would mediate the association between group (BPD vs. HC) and ratings of trustworthiness, likeability, and cooperativeness.

Results

Ratings for positive affect display were significantly lower and those for negative affect display significantly higher for BPD versus HC targets, whereas eye contact did not differ significantly between groups. In multiple mediation models, positive affect display significantly mediated the association between group and trustworthiness/likeability, whereas negative affect display only mediated the association between group and likeability. None of the individual cues was a significant mediator of the association between group and cooperation.

Conclusions

We emphasize therapeutic possibilities to improve positive affect display –and thus overall first impressions– to increase the chances of forming social bonds for BPD individuals.

Background

Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is a serious mental illness that afflicts between 1 and 3% of the adult population and starts to manifest in late childhood or early adolescence [1, 2]. Individuals with BPD have intense and rapidly changing emotions, tend to show impulsive and self-harming behaviors, and suffer from interpersonal problems [1]. The present study focuses on factors that contribute to interpersonal problems in BPD, which represent a core symptom of the disorder and are among the slowest BPD symptoms to remit [3, 4]. Interpersonal dysfunction in BPD manifests in various ways, but prominent examples include small social networks [5], high levels of romantic relationship dysfunction [6], and extreme loneliness [7].

In light of the central role interpersonal problems play in BPD, it is not surprising that much research has been devoted to identifying factors that contribute to their manifestation. Central factors that have been identified include impairments in social cognition, deficits in cooperative behavior, and functional neuronal alterations (for an overview, see [8,9,10]). Beyond these factors pertaining to altered processes on the part of the BPD individual, recent studies have also suggested that naïve raters form negative first impressions of those with BPD. Specifically, there is evidence that naïve raters tend to view BPD individuals negatively, which could contribute to interpersonal problems by way of negative behavior towards the BPD individual.

Most studies that assess how BPD individuals are perceived by others have focused on populations of health professionals and their attitudes towards BPD patients. These studies show that health professionals tend to evaluate BPD patients more negatively than other patient groups on dimensions such as likeability (for a review, see [11]). Beyond these, there is also a small number of studies that assessed how BPD individuals are perceived at zero-acquaintance (i.e. in a ‘first impression’ type situation where the rater has no knowledge of the individual and no prior interactions with them), that is, when their diagnosis is not known. A series of studies by Daros and colleagues [12] showed that, based on photographs, BPD individuals were evaluated as more mentally and physically ill than healthy participants at zero-acquaintance. Additionally, BPD individuals were attributed more negative emotions and less happiness than healthy control individuals in this sample. Additional research on zero-acquaintance judgments of BPD individuals was conducted by Oltmanns and colleagues [13] as well as Friedman and colleagues [14], who collected large target samples of military personnel and over-sampled for personality disorder features. Short video sequences of targets talking about ‘things they enjoy doing’ were shown to student raters. Raters evaluated targets with high BPD features low on likeability, extraversion, agreeableness, openness, and conscientiousness, and high on neuroticism. However, since both studies eventually included only six individuals that fulfilled a formal BPD diagnosis, these findings are inconclusive for the clinical BPD population.

Building on these studies, our group has recently published a study using the ‘Thin Slices’ paradigm [15], in which we presented short videos of 52 BPD and age- and gender-matched healthy control (HC) participants (‘targets’) to two groups of student raters (N1 = 92, N2 = 44). These raters evaluated targets on the dimensions trustworthiness, likeability, and cooperativeness [16]. In both samples, BPD targets were rated as significantly less trustworthy and less likeable, and in one sample also as less cooperative. Notably, these findings were present without raters knowing anything about the targets’ mental health status, and therefore the influence of top-down processes such as stereotypes about mental illness was excluded. Moreover, the effects were markedly larger when we presented videos without (vs. with) the audio trace. This led us to conclude that raters must have relied on visual cues to form their judgments to a substantial degree. Following from this, we developed the ensuing research question, which visual cues raters could have used to form their negative judgments about BPD targets. The current study addressed this research question, specifically focusing on observable behaviors as possible cues. We focused on specific behaviors since these could potentially be modified in a therapeutic context, aiming to counter negative first impressions.

The present study

To select probable cues for the trustworthiness, likeability and cooperativeness judgments obtained in the previous study [16], we did an extensive literature search on previous Thin Slices studies. Our search revealed affect expression as a central cue that raters consistently used to form judgments on constructs that measure some form of cooperativeness. Multiple studies found that raters use the intensity and frequency of positive versus negative affect expression (especially overt smiling) as a cue for agreeableness and altruism [17,18,19,20,21,22,23], and also cooperative behavior in economic games [22]. Beyond this, the display of positive facial affect was found to be a cue for likeability [20, 24], and for trustworthiness ratings [24,25,26,27,28]. In addition to facial affect display, the level of eye contact or looking straight into the camera emerged as another cue from the literature. In previous studies, the degree of eye contact was positively associated with agreeableness ratings [17, 29] as well as likeability [30,31,32] and trustworthiness ratings [33].

Based on this research, we selected positive affect (PA) display, negative affect (NA) display, and eye contact (EC) as potential cues for trustworthiness, likeability, and cooperativeness. Specifically, we expected these cues to mediate the observed relationship between BPD diagnostic status and poorer overall evaluation of trustworthiness, likeability and cooperativeness. Our assumption was also based on previous evidence that BPD individuals show low levels of PA display in experimental contexts, such as in reaction to PA induction [34], during a cyberball game [35], or in response to emotional pictures [36]. Likewise, BPD individuals displayed more NA (measured using facial electromyography) in previous studies when viewing emotional pictures [36,37,38] and during a problem-focused interview [39]. Previous evidence on reduced EC in BPD is lacking and therefore the examination of this cue is to be considered somewhat exploratory.

In sum, previous studies have provided first evidence that BPD individuals are evaluated negatively at a zero-acquaintance level, but it is yet unknown what these negative evaluations are based on. We identified PA and NA display as well as EC as potential cues for negative evaluations and expected BPD individuals to display more NA, less PA, and less EC. We expected that these observable cues would mediate the association between BPD status and negative evaluations on the traits trustworthiness, likeability, and cooperativeness.

Methods

Participants

A total of 101 participants were recruited via a participant pool of the University of Mannheim. Participants were between 18 and 55 years old (M = 23.7, SD = 4.5) and the majority of participants were female (60.4%), held a university entrance level degree (98.0%), and were students (84.2%) with a monthly income of less than 1000 Euros (76.2%).

Procedure

Ethics approval for this study was granted by the Medical Ethics Committee II of the medical faculty Mannheim at Heidelberg University (protocol no. 2013-654 N-MA). At the beginning of the study, participants received detailed information about the study procedure and instructions. All participants gave written informed consent prior to participation. After providing demographic information, participants saw the 52 target videos and rated several nonverbal cues after each video. Participants enrolled in psychology majors received course credit for their participation.

Material

Video-material

Detailed information on the generation of the video material is presented in Hepp et al. [16]. The video material comprised videos of 26 BPD and 26 age- and gender-matched HC participants. Participants were diagnosed by experienced clinicians using the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV SCID-I [40] and the international personality disorder examination IPDE [41]. Inclusion criteria were a current DSM-IV BPD diagnosis for the BPD group and the absence of any current or lifetime mental disorder or personality disorder for the HC group. In both groups, 46% of participants were men, and age did not differ significantly between the groups (MBPD = 32.2, SDBPD = 7.7; MHC = 31.9, SDHC = 8.0; t(50) = − 0.11, p = .916). Further demographic and diagnostic information on the target sample is presented in Hepp et al. [16].

Target participants were filmed while talking about their personal preferences (their favourite book, colour, movie, car, animal, food, hobby, and holiday destination). For the current study, videos were cut at 30 s and presented without the audio trace to exclude potential effects of speech content or prosody. Before the video-recording, target participants played an economic game called dictator game [42]. Participants were given an envelope containing 5 Euros in 50 cent coins and instructed to divide this money between themselves and an ‘unknown third person’. Participants extracted the money under complete anonymity and the recipients of the money remained unknown and had no way to react to the allocation. The amount of money shared in the dictator game is typically seen as an indicator of active cooperation or altruism. BPD and HC targets did not differ in the amount of money they actually shared, t(50) = 0.35, p = .727, d = 0.10.

Trustworthiness, likeability, and cooperativeness ratings

Trustworthiness, likeability, and cooperativeness (estimated money shared in dictator game) ratings for each target were obtained in a sample of 44 rater participants, which has previously been reported in Hepp et al. [16]. For the current study, we used the average ratings for each category per target. Ratings in all three categories were lower for BPD than for HC targets. BPD targets were evaluated as less trustworthy (MBPD = 2.32, SDBPD = 0.48, MHC = 2.93, SDHC = 0.51, t(50) = − 4.44, p < .001, d = 1.23), less likeable (MBPD = 2.16, SDBPD = 0.60, MHC = 2.78, SDHC = 0.71, t(50) = − 3.40, p = .001, d = 0.94), and less cooperative in terms of estimated money allocation in the dictator game (MBPD = 2.02, SDBPD = 0.27, MHC = 2.27, SDHC = 0.31, t(50) = − 3.17, p = .003, d = 0.88).

Cue ratings

Participants in the present study saw all 52 target videos and rated targets on nine different cues on a scale from 0 (“not at all”) to 5 (“very much”). The selection of cues included the three cues of interest for the current study: PA display (“The person often showed positive emotions, e.g. smiled.”), NA display (“The person often showed negative emotions, e.g. frowned”), and EC (“The person often looked directly into the camera”). In addition to these cues, we collected data on six further cues including “The person seemed distant and cool”, “The person seemed relaxed and confident”, “The person seemed educated and cultivated”, “The person is attractive”, “The person had a feminine appearance”, and “The person had a masculine appearance”. These variables were not related to the purpose of the current study, because they are not descriptors of a specific behaviour of the target, but rather pertain to global impressions. They are, thus, at a different level of granularity than the observable behaviours PA, NA and EC. With the purpose in mind of identifying directly modifiable behaviours, we have only analysed cues at the highest level of granularity and excluded variables that represented more global impressions. Though not analysed herein, we provide data for all additional cues in the online supplemental materials.

Data analysis

To assess whether our previous finding that BPD individuals are seen as less trustworthy, less likeable, and less cooperative at zero acquaintance could be explained by PA, NA, and EC, we averaged each cue rating per target video and employed three mediation models using path analyses. We specified PA, NA, and EC (jointly) as mediators of the association between target group (BPD vs. HC) and trustworthiness, likeability, and cooperativeness ratings. Analyses were conducted in R, using the sem function of the lavaan package [43] with conventional standard errors. For the indirect effects, we additionally computed bootstrapped bias-corrected confidence intervals (CIboot) using the adjusted bootstrap percentile method.

Results

Descriptive statistics for the cue ratings by target group are presented in Table 1. As expected, BPD targets were rated as showing less PA, and more NA than HC targets. However, target groups did not differ significantly in perceived EC (although the Bayes factor for this test was inconclusive). Results of the mediation models presented in Fig. 1 corroborate this picture, showing that group significantly predicted PA and NA, but not EC. Moreover, NA significantly predicted only likeability ratings, whereas PA predicted all three constructs, trustworthiness, likeability and cooperativeness. EC was not a significant predictor of any of the three criteria. In the context of the three mediators, group had a significant (negative) direct effect on trustworthiness, but not on likeability or cooperativeness. In other words, whether a target participant was in the BPD or the HC group significantly predicted how trustworthy, but not how likeable and cooperative they were rated, when PA, NA, and EC were statistically adjusted for.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics by target group for the cues positive affect (PA) display, negative affect (NA) display, and eye contact (EC)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Unstandardized estimates of the path analyses that link BPD group to trustworthiness (panel a), likeability (panel b), and estimated amount of money shared in the dictator game (panel c) through the three mediating variables NA display, PA display, and eye contact (including a direct effect of group). Figure note. BPD = Borderline Personality Disorder, NA = negative affect, PA = positive affect. Significance indicated as * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

Indirect effects indicated that the group-trustworthiness association was significantly mediated by PA (b = − 0.22, CIboot = [− 0.52; − 0.04], p = .037) and NA display (b = − 0.20, CIboot = [− 0.47; − 0.03], p = .040) but not EC (b = 0.02, CIboot = [− 0.03; 0.16], p = .490). The total indirect effect for the multiple mediation was significant (b = − 0.39, CIboot = [− 0.65; − 0.19], p = .001).

The group-likeability association was significantly mediated only by PA display (b = − 0.40, CIboot = [− 0.82; − 0.13], p = .013), but not NA (b = − 0.13, CIboot = [− 0.40; 0.11], p = .247) or EC (b = 0.03, CIboot = [− 0.04; 0.21], p = .428). The total indirect effect for the multiple mediation was significant (b = − 0.49, CIboot = [− 0.83; − 0.21], p = .002).

The association between target group and estimated cooperativeness (i.e. money allocation in the dictator game) was not significantly mediated by either cue individually (PA: b = − 0.11, CIboot = [− 0.28; 0.00], p = .094; NA: b = − 0.06, CIboot = [− 0.23; 0.07], p = .345; EC: b = 0.00, CIboot = [− 0.05; 0.06], p = .950) but the total indirect effect indicated a significant overall mediation (b = − 0.17, CIboot = [− 0.33; − 0.06], p = .004).

Discussion

The present study aimed to address the question why individuals with BPD are evaluated more negatively than healthy individuals at zero-acquaintance [12,13,14, 16]. We hypothesized that negative zero-acquaintance evaluations would be influenced by non-verbal cue displays by targets. To address this question, we used a target dataset with videos of 26 BPD and 26 age- and gender-matched HC individuals. In a previous study using the same target set, we observed that BPD targets were evaluated as less trustworthy, likeable, and cooperative [16]. Based on an extensive literature search, we identified PA display, NA display, and EC as likely nonverbal cues for negative ratings on the dimensions trustworthiness, likeability and cooperativeness. In the present study, we collected ratings of PA, NA, and EC for the 52 target videos from 101 raters. Next, we specified these ratings as mediators of the association between target group (BPD vs. HC) and trustworthiness, likeability as well as cooperativeness.

We found that BPD targets were rated as showing less PA and more NA than HC targets, which corroborates a number of previous findings [12, 34,35,36,37,38,39]. Importantly, PA display mediated the association between BPD group membership and likeability as well as between BPD group and trustworthiness. In contrast, NA display only mediated the association between target group and trustworthiness. Contrary to our hypothesis, the amount of EC did not significantly differ between the target groups and consequently did not mediate any of these associations. Moreover, neither cue individually mediated the group-cooperativeness association.

The importance of PA display for creating positive impressions on a range of global traits has previously been discussed (e.g., [25]), in the sense that PA signals overall approachability and encourages social bonds. Relating this back to the BPD population, and especially to interpersonal problems in BPD, it seems likely that low rates of PA display and associated poorer first impressions contribute to social isolation and interpersonal problems, because it might influence the initial behavior of an interaction partner. In other words, it seems likely that interpersonal problems in BPD are not only a result of impairments on the side of the BPD individual, but also of negative first impressions that other people form about those with BPD.

Limitations and implications

A central limitation of the cue based design we employed herein is that it relies on subjective ratings of cues (even though this is a common practice with Thin Slices studies, e.g. 17). The sample we report herein rated BPD targets as showing less PA and more NA than HC targets, but this does not constitute a definite, objective measure of their affect display. For this reason, more recent approaches to cue based designs aim towards more objective measures of cues, for instance by using electromyography, or software that detects facial affect display based on patterns of muscle activation in the faceFootnote 1.

Additionally, the set of cues we chose, while strongly informed by previous evidence, is clearly not exhaustive and there are a multitude of other non-verbal behaviors raters could have relied on. Therefore, future studies are needed to replicate the present findings and extend the set of cues. Moreover, mediation analysis as used herein does not imply that significant mediators are at all causal or that another mediation model would not better explain the associations between group and trustworthiness/likeability/cooperativeness (see [44]). This again stresses the need for increasing objectivity and reliability of the cue ratings and extending the set of cues, or ideally even manipulating cues in experimental designs.

Nonetheless, affect expression provides a first, very clear target to address in further studies, and possibly also in therapeutic contexts. Therefore, it would be desirable to further refine the knowledge about how exactly affect expression informs global evaluations. Herein, we asked participants how ‘often’ targets showed behaviors such as smiling or frowning, thus assessing most likely the frequency of a certain display of affect. Other studies could distinguish between frequency, intensity, and duration, as these might have differential effects. For example, it would be helpful to know whether a brief but very intense smile could ‘compensate’ the influence of multiple minor frowns on the global expression.

In addition to further researching different types of cues and their relative relevance, future studies should address the question of diagnostic specificity. In the present study, we included only one clinical group of BPD individuals and thus cannot conclude whether the findings we made are an effect of specific BPD pathology, or psychopathology in general. Moreover, the target sample we report suffered from substantial comorbidity, which is common in BPD samples (e.g., [2]), but also entails the limitation that major comorbid conditions such as depression could have contributed to the observed lower rate of PA and more NA expression. Therefore, at this point, it would be premature to argue that our findings are specific to BPD. However, although few social bonds and interpersonal problems afflict patients of various disorders, it seems reasonable to study BPD individuals, as they are very prominently affected. Eventually, the finding we report herein may actually apply to other types of psychopathology as well and addressing affect display may be helpful for a range of patient groups.

The topic of affect display could be integrated in existing treatments of BPD such as dialectical behavior therapy [45], either by way of including it in skills training, or in other forms of social competence training. In any scenario, the inclusion of video-feedback could be particularly beneficial. The current results suggest a focus on PA display, as it had the strongest effects on global attributions of trustworthiness and likeability. Suppression of NA display, in contrast, may be a two-edged sword. On the one hand, the present findings and conceptualizations of NA as a withdrawal or threat signal would suggest including reduction of NA expression in therapeutic approaches. On the other hand, suppression of NA is (even outside of clinical samples) largely considered to be a poorly effective emotion regulation strategy that can actually increase subjective and physiological arousal (e.g., [46]). Therefore, therapeutic interventions to increase PA display seem the most likely starting point based on the limited evidence that is currently available.

Conclusion

The present findings provide first evidence that a lack of facial PA expression and, to a lesser degree, also a high level of facial NA expression could contribute to negative evaluations of BPD individuals at zero-acquaintance. Thus, fostering BPD patients’ PA expression in therapeutic settings may help them elicit a more positive first impression in others. In our opinion, replication of the present findings and extension to a broader set of cues pertaining to non-verbal behaviors is the necessary next step. Any attempts to modify impression management in therapeutic settings can only come after this. Nonetheless, we hope that the potential benefit for patient populations will encourage other researchers to follow up on this question. If it were possible to teach patients to improve their impression management by way of learning to express PA more readily, and thus elicit a more positive first impression, this could increase BPD individuals’ opportunities to form social bonds and improve their chances at maintaining them.

Notes

  1. Since we wanted target participants to provide a video Thin Slice that was as naturalistic as possible, we chose not to attach electrodes to their faces or instruct them to restrict their movement, which would have been necessary to obtain reliable scores of facial affect based on computer algorithms.

Abbreviations

BF10 :

Jeffrey-Zellner-Siow Bayes factor

BPD:

Borderline Personality Disorder

CIboot :

Bootstrapped bias-corrected confidence interval

DSM-IV:

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

EC:

Eye contact

HC:

Healthy control participants

IPDE:

International personality disorder examination

NA:

Negative affect

PA:

Positive affect

SCID-I:

Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Tomko RL, Trull TJ, Wood PK, Sher KJ. Characteristics of borderline personality disorder in a community sample: comorbidity, treatment utilization, and general functioning. J Pers Dis. 2014;28(5):734–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Gunderson JG, Stout RL, McGlashan TH, Shea MT, Morey LC, Grilo CM, et al. Ten-year course of borderline personality disorder: psychopathology and function from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(8):827–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Reich DB, Fitzmaurice G. The 10-year course of psychosocial functioning among patients with borderline personality disorder and axis II comparison subjects. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;122(2):103–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Stepp SD, Pilkonis PA, Yaggi KE, Morse JQ, Feske U. Interpersonal and emotional experiences of social interactions in borderline personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2009;197(7):484–91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Hill J, Stepp SD, Wan MW, Hope H, Morse JQ, Steele M, et al. Attachment, borderline personality, and romantic relationship dysfunction. J Pers Dis. 2011;25(6):789–805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Liebke L, Bungert M, Thome J, Hauschild S, Gescher DM, Schmahl C, et al. Loneliness, social networks, and social functioning in borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord. 2017;8(4):349–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Daros AR, Zakzanis KK, Ruocco AC. Facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med. 2013;43(9):1953–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Jeung H, Schwieren C, Herpertz SC. Rationality and self-interest as economic-exchange strategy in borderline personality disorder: game theory, social preferences, and interpersonal behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:849–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Schulze L, Schmahl C, Niedtfeld I. Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: a multimodal meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79(2):97–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Aviram RB, Brodsky BS, Stanley B. Borderline personality disorder, stigma, and treatment implications. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2006;14(5):249–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Daros AR, Ruocco AC, Rule NO. Identifying mental disorder from the faces of women with borderline personality disorder. J Nonverbal Behav. 2016;40(4):255–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Oltmanns TF, Friedman JN, Fiedler ER, Turkheimer E. Perceptions of people with personality disorders based on thin slices of behavior. J Res Pers. 2004;38(3):216–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Friedman JN, Oltmanns TF, Gleason ME, Turkheimer E. Mixed impressions: reactions of strangers to people with pathological personality traits. J Res Pers. 2006;40(4):395–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Ambady N, Rosenthal R. Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 1992;111(2):256–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hepp J, Störkel LM, Kieslich PJ, Schmahl C, Niedtfeld I. Negative evaluation of individuals with borderline personality disorder at zero acquaintance. Behav Res Ther. 2018;111:84–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Borkenau P, Liebler A. Trait inferences: sources of validity at zero acquaintance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;62(4):645–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Borkenau P, Liebler A. Observable attributes as manifestations and cues of personality and intelligence. J Pers. 1995;63(1):1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kenny DA, Horner C, Kashy DA. Chu L-c. Consensus at zero acquaintance: replication, behavioral cues, and stability. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;62(1):88–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Naumann LP, Vazire S, Rentfrow PJ, Gosling SD. Personality judgments based on physical appearance. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2009;35(12):1661–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Brown WM, Palameta B, Moore C. Are there nonverbal cues to commitment? An exploratory study using the zero-acquaintance video presentation paradigm. Evol Psychol. 2003;1(1):42–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Reed LI, Zeglen KN, Schmidt KL. Facial expressions as honest signals of cooperative intent in a one-shot anonymous Prisoner's dilemma game. Evol Hum Behav. 2012;33(3):200–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Tackman AM, Srivastava S. Social responses to expressive suppression: the role of personality judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2016;110(4):574–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Todorov A, Olson IR. Robust learning of affective trait associations with faces when the hippocampus is damaged, but not when the amygdala and temporal pole are damaged. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3(3):195–203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Todorov A, Said CP, Engell AD, Oosterhof NN. Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(12):455–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Oosterhof NN, Todorov A. Shared perceptual basis of emotional expressions and trustworthiness impressions from faces. Emot. 2009;9(1):128–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Caulfield F, Ewing L, Bank S, Rhodes G. Judging trustworthiness from faces: emotion cues modulate trustworthiness judgments in young children. Br J Psychol. 2016;107(3):503–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Ma DS, Correll J, Wittenbrink B. The Chicago face database: a free stimulus set of faces and norming data. Behav Res Methods. 2015;47(4):1122–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Larsen RJ, Shackelford TK. Gaze avoidance: personality and social judgments of people who avoid direct face-to-face contact. Pers Indiv Dif. 1996;21(6):907–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Kendon A, Cook M. The consistency of gaze patterns in social interaction. Br J Psychol. 1969;60(4):481–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Mason MF, Tatkow EP, Macrae CN. The look of love: gaze shifts and person perception. Psychol Sci. 2005;16(3):236–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Argyle M, Lefebvre L, Cook M. The meaning of five patterns of gaze. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1974;4(2):125–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Bayliss AP, Tipper SP. Predictive gaze cues and personality judgments: should eye trust you? Psychol Sci. 2006;17(6):514–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Renneberg B, Heyn K, Gebhard R, Bachmann S. Facial expression of emotions in borderline personality disorder and depression. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2005;36(3):183–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Staebler K, Renneberg B, Stopsack M, Fiedler P, Weiler M, Roepke S. Facial emotional expression in reaction to social exclusion in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med. 2011;41(9):1929–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Matzke B, Herpertz SC, Berger C, Fleischer M, Domes G. Facial reactions during emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: a facial electromyography study. Psychopathology. 2014;47(2):101–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Davies H, Wolz I, Leppanen J, Fernandez-Aranda F, Schmidt U, Tchanturia K. Facial expression to emotional stimuli in non-psychotic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;64:252–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Herpertz SC, Werth U, Lukas G, Qunaibi M, Schuerkens A, Kunert H-J, et al. Emotion in criminal offenders with psychopathy and borderline personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(8):737–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Peham D, Bock A, Schiestl C, Huber E, Zimmermann J, Kratzer D, et al. Facial affective behavior in mental disorder. J Nonverbal Behav. 2015;39(4):371–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Wittchen H, Zaudig M, Fydrich T. Strukturiertes klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Göttingen. Germany: Hogrefe; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Loranger A, Sartorius N, Andreoli A, Berger P, Buchheim P, Channabasavanna S, et al. Deutschsprachige Fassung der International Personality Disorder Examination: IPDE. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Forsythe R, Horowitz JL, Savin NE, Sefton M. Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games Econ Behav. 1994;6(3):347–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Fiedler K, Schott M, Meiser T. What mediation analysis can (not) do. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2011;47(6):1231–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Linehan MM. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;64(6):970–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Prof. Dr. Georg Alpers for the generous use of his lab space at the University of Mannheim and valuable feedback on the study design. We acknowledge financial support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft within the funding programme Open Access Publishing, by the Baden-Württemberg Ministry of Science, Research and the Arts and by Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant (NI 1591/1–1) of the German Research Foundation to IN.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available in the OSF repository, https://osf.io/tqbka/.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JH, SG, and IN conceived the study design. JH, SG, and LS collected the data. JH and PJK analyzed the data. JH drafted the manuscript and all authors provided critical revisions to the manuscript. All authors have approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johanna Hepp.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval for this study was granted by the Medical Ethics Committee II of the medical faculty Mannheim at Heidelberg University (protocol no. 2013-654 N-MA). All participants provided written, informed consent prior to participation.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hepp, J., Gebhardt, S., Kieslich, P.J. et al. Low positive affect display mediates the association between borderline personality disorder and negative evaluations at zero acquaintance. bord personal disord emot dysregul 6, 4 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-019-0103-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-019-0103-6

Keywords